These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

125 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24734895)

  • 1. Why the body comes first: effects of experimenter touch on infants' word finding.
    Seidl A; Tincoff R; Baker C; Cristia A
    Dev Sci; 2015 Jan; 18(1):155-64. PubMed ID: 24734895
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Words and possible words in early language acquisition.
    Marchetto E; Bonatti LL
    Cogn Psychol; 2013 Nov; 67(3):130-50. PubMed ID: 24041871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. A multimodal corpus of speech to infant and adult listeners.
    Johnson EK; Lahey M; Ernestus M; Cutler A
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2013 Dec; 134(6):EL534. PubMed ID: 25669300
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Multimodal infant-directed communication: how caregivers combine tactile and linguistic cues.
    Abu-Zhaya R; Seidl A; Cristia A
    J Child Lang; 2017 Sep; 44(5):1088-1116. PubMed ID: 27573414
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The beginnings of word segmentation in english-learning infants.
    Jusczyk PW; Houston DM; Newsome M
    Cogn Psychol; 1999; 39(3-4):159-207. PubMed ID: 10631011
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Infants' learning of novel words in a stochastic environment.
    Vouloumanos A; Werker JF
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1611-7. PubMed ID: 19899918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Infants' preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words.
    Jusczyk PW; Cutler A; Redanz NJ
    Child Dev; 1993 Jun; 64(3):675-87. PubMed ID: 8339688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Mothers' use of touch across infants' development and its implications for word learning: Evidence from Korean dyadic interactions.
    Ko ES; Abu-Zhaya R; Kim ES; Kim T; On KW; Kim H; Zhang BT; Seidl A
    Infancy; 2023 May; 28(3):597-618. PubMed ID: 36757022
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The use of social and salience cues in early word learning.
    Houston-Price C; Plunkett K; Duffy H
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2006 Sep; 95(1):27-55. PubMed ID: 16677668
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Exploring the "anchor word" effect in infants: Segmentation and categorisation of speech with and without high frequency words.
    Frost RLA; Dunn K; Christiansen MH; Gómez RL; Monaghan P
    PLoS One; 2020; 15(12):e0243436. PubMed ID: 33332419
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. (Non)words, (non)words, (non)words: evidence for a protolexicon during the first year of life.
    Ngon C; Martin A; Dupoux E; Cabrol D; Dutat M; Peperkamp S
    Dev Sci; 2013 Jan; 16(1):24-34. PubMed ID: 23278924
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Words and syllables in fluent speech segmentation by French-learning infants: an ERP study.
    Goyet L; de Schonen S; Nazzi T
    Brain Res; 2010 May; 1332():75-89. PubMed ID: 20331982
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Electrophysiological evidence for prelinguistic infants' word recognition in continuous speech.
    Kooijman V; Hagoort P; Cutler A
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2005 Jun; 24(1):109-16. PubMed ID: 15922163
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Infants' statistical word segmentation in an artificial language is linked to both parental speech input and reported production abilities.
    Hoareau M; Yeung HH; Nazzi T
    Dev Sci; 2019 Jul; 22(4):e12803. PubMed ID: 30681753
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Effects of prior phonotactic knowledge on infant word segmentation: the case of nonadjacent dependencies.
    Gonzalez-Gomez N; Nazzi T
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2013 Jun; 56(3):840-9. PubMed ID: 23275409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech.
    Jusczyk PW; Aslin RN
    Cogn Psychol; 1995 Aug; 29(1):1-23. PubMed ID: 7641524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: evidence from German and French infants.
    Höhle B; Bijeljac-Babic R; Herold B; Weissenborn J; Nazzi T
    Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Jun; 32(3):262-74. PubMed ID: 19427039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Acquisition of early words from single-word and sentential contexts.
    Trehub SE; Shenfield T
    Dev Sci; 2007 Mar; 10(2):190-8. PubMed ID: 17286843
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Emergence of Japanese infants' prosodic preferences in infant-directed vocabulary.
    Hayashi A; Mazuka R
    Dev Psychol; 2017 Jan; 53(1):28-37. PubMed ID: 27893237
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.