These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

243 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24791710)

  • 1. A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: the case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds.
    Blackwell KA; Chatham CH; Wiseheart M; Munakata Y
    Neuropsychologia; 2014 Sep; 62():356-64. PubMed ID: 24791710
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Two types of perseveration in the Dimension Change Card Sort task.
    Hanania R
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):325-36. PubMed ID: 20566206
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Automaticity of cognitive control: goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms.
    Verbruggen F; Logan GD
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2009 Sep; 35(5):1381-8. PubMed ID: 19686032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Different Executive Functions Support Different Kinds of Cognitive Flexibility: Evidence From 2-, 3-, and 4-Year-Olds.
    Blakey E; Visser I; Carroll DJ
    Child Dev; 2016; 87(2):513-26. PubMed ID: 26659697
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Differential contributions of set-shifting and monitoring to dual-task interference.
    Cooper RP; Wutke K; Davelaar EJ
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2012; 65(3):587-612. PubMed ID: 22182315
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Schizophrenia patients show task switching deficits consistent with N-methyl-d-aspartate system dysfunction but not global executive deficits: implications for pathophysiology of executive dysfunction in schizophrenia.
    Wylie GR; Clark EA; Butler PD; Javitt DC
    Schizophr Bull; 2010 May; 36(3):585-94. PubMed ID: 18835838
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The central executive as a search process: priming exploration and exploitation across domains.
    Hills TT; Todd PM; Goldstone RL
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2010 Nov; 139(4):590-609. PubMed ID: 21038983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Sustained selective attention predicts flexible switching in preschoolers.
    Benitez VL; Vales C; Hanania R; Smith LB
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Apr; 156():29-42. PubMed ID: 28024178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Inhibit, switch, and update: A within-subject fMRI investigation of executive control.
    Lemire-Rodger S; Lam J; Viviano JD; Stevens WD; Spreng RN; Turner GR
    Neuropsychologia; 2019 Sep; 132():107134. PubMed ID: 31299188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Task switching costs in preschool children and adults.
    Peng A; Kirkham NZ; Mareschal D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Aug; 172():59-72. PubMed ID: 29587131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching.
    Davidson MC; Amso D; Anderson LC; Diamond A
    Neuropsychologia; 2006; 44(11):2037-78. PubMed ID: 16580701
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Regular physical activity improves executive function during task switching in young adults.
    Kamijo K; Takeda Y
    Int J Psychophysiol; 2010 Mar; 75(3):304-11. PubMed ID: 20079771
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Cognitive and behavioral aspects of executive functions in children born very preterm.
    Ritter BC; Perrig W; Steinlin M; Everts R
    Child Neuropsychol; 2014 Mar; 20(2):129-44. PubMed ID: 23458400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task.
    Huizinga M; van der Molen MW
    Dev Neuropsychol; 2007; 31(2):193-215. PubMed ID: 17488216
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Carving executive control at its joints: Working memory capacity predicts stimulus-stimulus, but not stimulus-response, conflict.
    Meier ME; Kane MJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2015 Nov; 41(6):1849-72. PubMed ID: 26120774
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Relations between gross motor skills and executive functions, controlling for the role of information processing and lapses of attention in 8-10 year old children.
    van der Fels IMJ; Smith J; de Bruijn AGM; Bosker RJ; Königs M; Oosterlaan J; Visscher C; Hartman E
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(10):e0224219. PubMed ID: 31648263
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Developmental changes in decision making under risk: The role of executive functions and reasoning abilities in 8- to 19-year-old decision makers.
    Schiebener J; García-Arias M; García-Villamisar D; Cabanyes-Truffino J; Brand M
    Child Neuropsychol; 2015; 21(6):759-78. PubMed ID: 25027746
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Perceptual biases during cued task switching relate to decision process differences between children and adults.
    Martinez JE; Mack ML; Bauer JR; Roe MA; Church JA
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2018 Oct; 44(10):1603-1618. PubMed ID: 30024226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. When simple things are meaningful: working memory strength predicts children's cognitive flexibility.
    Blackwell KA; Cepeda NJ; Munakata Y
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Jun; 103(2):241-9. PubMed ID: 19278688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Bivalent task switching and memory load: Similar costs on reaction times, different costs on concurrent timing.
    Viau-Quesnel C; Fortin C
    Can J Exp Psychol; 2014 Sep; 68(3):194-203. PubMed ID: 25383477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 13.