BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

1138 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24969660)

  • 1. Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition.
    Fitch WT
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):329-64. PubMed ID: 24969660
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Implementation is crucial but must be neurobiologically grounded. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W. Tecumseh Fitch.
    Bornkessel-Schlesewsky I; Schlesewsky M; Small SL
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):365-6. PubMed ID: 24957290
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Decomposing dendrophilia. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W. Tecumseh Fitch.
    Honing H; Zuidema W
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):375-6. PubMed ID: 24998340
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Rethinking the cognitive revolution from a neural perspective: how overuse/misuse of the term 'cognition' and the neglect of affective controls in behavioral neuroscience could be delaying progress in understanding the BrainMind.
    Cromwell HC; Panksepp J
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2011 Oct; 35(9):2026-35. PubMed ID: 21345347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Cognitive theory and brain fact: insights for the future of cognitive neuroscience. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W. Tecumseh Fitch.
    Bowling D
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):377-9. PubMed ID: 25089022
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Evolutionary basis for human language. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by Tecumseh Fitch.
    Steedman M
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):382-8. PubMed ID: 25074443
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Representations, approximations, and limitations within a computational framework for cognitive science. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: Unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W. Tecumseh Fitch.
    Perfors A
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):369-70. PubMed ID: 24954729
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Parallel Distributed Processing at 25: further explorations in the microstructure of cognition.
    Rogers TT; McClelland JL
    Cogn Sci; 2014 Aug; 38(6):1024-77. PubMed ID: 25087578
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Learning, epigenetics, and computation: an extension on Fitch's proposal. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W. Tecumseh Fitch.
    Okanoya K
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):389-90. PubMed ID: 25027505
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Brains are not just neurons. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by Fitch.
    Huber L
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):373-4. PubMed ID: 24908289
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Beyond single-level accounts: the role of cognitive architectures in cognitive scientific explanation.
    Cooper RP; Peebles D
    Top Cogn Sci; 2015 Apr; 7(2):243-58. PubMed ID: 25726919
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Attending to the forest and the trees. Reply to comments on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition".
    Fitch WT
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):391-9. PubMed ID: 25112825
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Naturalizing cognition: the integration of cognitive science and biology.
    Glenberg AM
    Curr Biol; 2006 Sep; 16(18):R802-4. PubMed ID: 16979548
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The forest for the trees. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W.T. Fitch.
    Johnson M
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):371-2. PubMed ID: 25027504
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The software/wetware distinction. Comment on "Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: unifying approaches from cognitive neuroscience and comparative cognition" by W. Tecumseh Fitch.
    Dennett D
    Phys Life Rev; 2014 Sep; 11(3):367-8. PubMed ID: 24998042
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Empiricists are from Venus, modelers are from Mars: Reconciling experimental and computational approaches in cognitive neuroscience.
    Cowell RA; Bussey TJ; Saksida LM
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2012 Nov; 36(10):2371-9. PubMed ID: 22960226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Beyond the "Conceptual Nervous System": Can computational cognitive neuroscience transform learning theory?
    Soto FA
    Behav Processes; 2019 Oct; 167():103908. PubMed ID: 31381986
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Cognitive computational neuroscience.
    Kriegeskorte N; Douglas PK
    Nat Neurosci; 2018 Sep; 21(9):1148-1160. PubMed ID: 30127428
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Modeling developmental cognitive neuroscience.
    Westermann G; Sirois S; Shultz TR; Mareschal D
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2006 May; 10(5):227-32. PubMed ID: 16603407
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Preface to the special issue: computational cognitive neuroscience.
    Becker S
    Brain Res; 2008 Apr; 1202():1-2. PubMed ID: 17658487
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 57.