BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

158 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24994991)

  • 1. Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: the role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation.
    Hamlin JK
    Front Psychol; 2014; 5():614. PubMed ID: 24994991
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The limits of early social evaluation: 9-month-olds fail to generate social evaluations of individuals who behave inconsistently.
    Steckler CM; Woo BM; Hamlin JK
    Cognition; 2017 Oct; 167():255-265. PubMed ID: 28395908
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: gazing toward one's goal drives infants' preferences for Helpers over Hinderers in the hill paradigm.
    Hamlin JK
    Front Psychol; 2014; 5():1563. PubMed ID: 25688216
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Mechanisms of social evaluation in infancy: A preregistered exploration of infants' eye-movement and pupillary responses to prosocial and antisocial events.
    Tan E; Hamlin JK
    Infancy; 2022 Mar; 27(2):255-276. PubMed ID: 34873821
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Infants' neural responses to helping and hindering scenarios.
    Tan E; Hamlin JK
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2022 Apr; 54():101095. PubMed ID: 35276494
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The early emergence of sociomoral evaluation: infants prefer prosocial others.
    Van de Vondervoort JW; Hamlin JK
    Curr Opin Psychol; 2018 Apr; 20():77-81. PubMed ID: 28858770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Preschoolers' social and moral judgments of third-party helpers and hinderers align with infants' social evaluations.
    Van de Vondervoort JW; Hamlin JK
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Dec; 164():136-151. PubMed ID: 28822295
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others.
    Hamlin JK; Wynn K
    Cogn Dev; 2011 Jan; 26(1):30-39. PubMed ID: 21499550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The Development of Infants' Sensitivity to Behavioral Intentions when Inferring Others' Social Preferences.
    Lee YE; Yun JE; Kim EY; Song HJ
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(9):e0135588. PubMed ID: 26383160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. From understanding others' needs to prosocial action: Motor and social abilities promote infants' helping.
    Köster M; Itakura S; Omori M; Kärtner J
    Dev Sci; 2019 Nov; 22(6):e12804. PubMed ID: 30706665
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Negativity bias in infants' expectations about agents' dispositions.
    Chae JJK; Song HJ
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2018 Nov; 36(4):620-633. PubMed ID: 29717497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Bonobos Prefer Individuals that Hinder Others over Those that Help.
    Krupenye C; Hare B
    Curr Biol; 2018 Jan; 28(2):280-286.e5. PubMed ID: 29307556
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Social evaluation of intentional, truly accidental, and negligently accidental helpers and harmers by 10-month-old infants.
    Woo BM; Steckler CM; Le DT; Hamlin JK
    Cognition; 2017 Nov; 168():154-163. PubMed ID: 28688284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Neural signatures to prosocial and antisocial interactions in young infants.
    Licht V; Addabbo M; Nava E; Turati C
    Soc Neurosci; 2023 Aug; 18(4):207-217. PubMed ID: 37610285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Feeling out a link between feeling and infant sociomoral evaluation.
    Steckler CM; Liberman Z; Van de Vondervoort JW; Slevinsky J; Le DT; Hamlin JK
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2018 Sep; 36(3):482-500. PubMed ID: 29285770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Folic acid supplementation and malaria susceptibility and severity among people taking antifolate antimalarial drugs in endemic areas.
    Crider K; Williams J; Qi YP; Gutman J; Yeung L; Mai C; Finkelstain J; Mehta S; Pons-Duran C; Menéndez C; Moraleda C; Rogers L; Daniels K; Green P
    Cochrane Database Syst Rev; 2022 Feb; 2(2022):. PubMed ID: 36321557
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Probing the Strength of Infants' Preference for Helpers over Hinderers: Two Replication Attempts of Hamlin and Wynn (2011).
    Salvadori E; Blazsekova T; Volein A; Karap Z; Tatone D; Mascaro O; Csibra G
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(11):e0140570. PubMed ID: 26565412
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Toddlers Prefer Agents Who Help Those Facing Harder Tasks.
    Woo BM; Liu S; Gweon H; Spelke ES
    Open Mind (Camb); 2024; 8():483-499. PubMed ID: 38665545
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. How do toddlers evaluate defensive actions toward third parties?
    Geraci A
    Infancy; 2020 Nov; 25(6):910-926. PubMed ID: 33022888
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.