These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

137 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25079950)

  • 1. Comparison of active and purely visual performance in a multiple-string means-end task in infants.
    Rat-Fischer L; O'Regan JK; Fagard J
    Cognition; 2014 Oct; 133(1):304-16. PubMed ID: 25079950
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Vertical string-pulling in green jays (Cyanocorax yncas).
    Manrique HM; Molina AC; Posada S; Colell M
    Behav Processes; 2017 Jul; 140():74-80. PubMed ID: 28412529
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Means to an end: neotropical parrots manage to pull strings to meet their goals.
    Schuck-Paim C; Borsari A; Ottoni EB
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):287-301. PubMed ID: 18766389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Predictive gaze shifts elicited during observed and performed actions in 10-month-old infants and adults.
    Rosander K; von Hofsten C
    Neuropsychologia; 2011 Aug; 49(10):2911-7. PubMed ID: 21722655
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Visual experience influences 12-month-old infants' perception of goal-directed actions of others.
    Myowa-Yamakoshi M; Kawakita Y; Okanda M; Takeshita H
    Dev Psychol; 2011 Jul; 47(4):1042-9. PubMed ID: 21604864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Perception and production of object-related grasping in 6-month-olds.
    Daum MM; Prinz W; Aschersleben G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Apr; 108(4):810-8. PubMed ID: 21092981
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. An investigation into the cognition behind spontaneous string pulling in New Caledonian crows.
    Taylor AH; Medina FS; Holzhaider JC; Hearne LJ; Hunt GR; Gray RD
    PLoS One; 2010 Feb; 5(2):e9345. PubMed ID: 20179759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Production and perception of contralateral reaching: a close link by 12 months of age.
    Melzer A; Prinz W; Daum MM
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Jun; 35(3):570-9. PubMed ID: 22728337
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Infants' understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: Interrelations among violation of expectancy and imitation tasks.
    Olineck KM; Poulin-Dubois D
    Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Dec; 32(4):404-15. PubMed ID: 19619899
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Action plans used in action observation.
    Flanagan JR; Johansson RS
    Nature; 2003 Aug; 424(6950):769-71. PubMed ID: 12917683
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Domestic cats (Felis catus) do not show causal understanding in a string-pulling task.
    Whitt E; Douglas M; Osthaus B; Hocking I
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Sep; 12(5):739-43. PubMed ID: 19449193
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Infant temperament and information processing in a visual categorization task.
    Vonderlin E; Pahnke J; Pauen S
    Infant Behav Dev; 2008 Dec; 31(4):559-69. PubMed ID: 18804872
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. What activates the human mirror neuron system during observation of artificial movements: bottom-up visual features or top-down intentions?
    Engel A; Burke M; Fiehler K; Bien S; Rösler F
    Neuropsychologia; 2008; 46(7):2033-42. PubMed ID: 18339409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy.
    Sommerville JA; Woodward AL
    Cognition; 2005 Feb; 95(1):1-30. PubMed ID: 15629472
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. [Comparative study of the ability to solve a string-pulling task in Corvidae].
    Bagotskaia MS; Smirnova AA; Zorina ZA
    Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova; 2010; 60(3):321-9. PubMed ID: 20737894
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. [Corvidae are able to understand the logical structure in string-pulling tasks].
    Bagotskaia MS; Smirnova AA; Zorina ZA
    Zh Vyssh Nerv Deiat Im I P Pavlova; 2010; 60(5):543-51. PubMed ID: 21260978
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) fail to show understanding of means-end connections in a string-pulling task.
    Osthaus B; Lea SE; Slater AM
    Anim Cogn; 2005 Jan; 8(1):37-47. PubMed ID: 15338446
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Visual experience enhances infants' use of task-relevant information in an action task.
    Wang SH; Kohne L
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Nov; 43(6):1513-22. PubMed ID: 18020828
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Eye movements and visuospatial problem solving: identifying separable phases of complex cognition.
    Kaller CP; Rahm B; Bolkenius K; Unterrainer JM
    Psychophysiology; 2009 Jul; 46(4):818-30. PubMed ID: 19490515
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Executive functioning demands of the object retrieval task for 8- month-old infants.
    Noland JS
    Child Neuropsychol; 2008 Nov; 14(6):504-9. PubMed ID: 18608230
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.