These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

132 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25080368)

  • 21. Apes' use of iconic cues in the object-choice task.
    Herrmann E; Melis AP; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Apr; 9(2):118-30. PubMed ID: 16395566
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Can monkeys (Macaca mulatta) represent invisible displacement?
    Filion CM; Washburn DA; Gulledge JP
    J Comp Psychol; 1996 Dec; 110(4):386-95. PubMed ID: 8956508
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. The use of a displacement device negatively affects the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) in visible object displacement tasks.
    Müller CA; Riemer S; Range F; Huber L
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Aug; 128(3):240-50. PubMed ID: 24611641
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Orangutans (Pongo spp.) do not spontaneously share benefits with familiar conspecifics in a choice paradigm.
    Kim Y; Martinez L; Choe JC; Lee DJ; Tomonaga M
    Primates; 2015 Apr; 56(2):193-200. PubMed ID: 25739582
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) point to inform a human about the location of a tool.
    Zimmermann F; Zemke F; Call J; Gómez JC
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):347-58. PubMed ID: 18953583
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. A nonverbal false belief task: the performance of children and great apes.
    Call J; Tomasello M
    Child Dev; 1999; 70(2):381-95. PubMed ID: 10218261
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Object permanence in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus).
    Mendes N; Huber L
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Mar; 118(1):103-12. PubMed ID: 15008678
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Developments in infants' search for displaced objects.
    Sophian C; Sage S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1983 Feb; 35(1):143-60. PubMed ID: 6827214
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Young children do not require perceptual-motor feedback to solve Aesop's Fable tasks.
    Miller R; Jelbert SA; Loissel E; Taylor AH; Clayton NS
    PeerJ; 2017; 5():e3484. PubMed ID: 28729951
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Object permanence in the pigeon (Columba livia): Insertion of a delay prior to choice facilitates visible- and invisible-displacement accuracy.
    Zentall TR; Raley OL
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 Feb; 133(1):132-139. PubMed ID: 30382709
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Manual function and performance in humans, gorillas, and orangutans during the same tool use task.
    Bardo A; Cornette R; Borel A; Pouydebat E
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2017 Dec; 164(4):821-836. PubMed ID: 28941286
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Object permanence in domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) and gray wolves (Canis lupus).
    Fiset S; Plourde V
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 May; 127(2):115-27. PubMed ID: 23106804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Can orangutans (Pongo abelii) infer tool functionality?
    Mulcahy NJ; Schubiger MN
    Anim Cogn; 2014 May; 17(3):657-69. PubMed ID: 24132413
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The ligamentum teres femoris in orangutans.
    Muchlinski MN; Hammond AS; Deane AS; Purcell M; Hemingway HW; Hantke G; Pastor F; Garrosa M; Hartstone-Rose A
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2018 Nov; 167(3):684-690. PubMed ID: 30132799
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Morphometric, Behavioral, and Genomic Evidence for a New Orangutan Species.
    Nater A; Mattle-Greminger MP; Nurcahyo A; Nowak MG; de Manuel M; Desai T; Groves C; Pybus M; Sonay TB; Roos C; Lameira AR; Wich SA; Askew J; Davila-Ross M; Fredriksson G; de Valles G; Casals F; Prado-Martinez J; Goossens B; Verschoor EJ; Warren KS; Singleton I; Marques DA; Pamungkas J; Perwitasari-Farajallah D; Rianti P; Tuuga A; Gut IG; Gut M; Orozco-terWengel P; van Schaik CP; Bertranpetit J; Anisimova M; Scally A; Marques-Bonet T; Meijaard E; Krützen M
    Curr Biol; 2017 Nov; 27(22):3487-3498.e10. PubMed ID: 29103940
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Tracking and inferring spatial rotation by children and great apes.
    Okamoto-Barth S; Call J
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Sep; 44(5):1396-408. PubMed ID: 18793071
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Orangutans (Pongo abelii) "play the odds": information-seeking strategies in relation to cost, risk, and benefit.
    Marsh HL; MacDonald SE
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Aug; 126(3):263-278. PubMed ID: 22023265
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Rotational displacement skills in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).
    Hughes KD; Santos LR
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Nov; 126(4):421-32. PubMed ID: 22866770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii) understand connectivity in the skewered grape tool task.
    Mulcahy NJ; Schubiger MN; Suddendorf T
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Feb; 127(1):109-13. PubMed ID: 22686164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Object permanence in lemurs.
    Deppe AM; Wright PC; Szelistowski WA
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):381-8. PubMed ID: 18936991
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.