These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

184 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25143037)

  • 1. Animal vocal sequences: not the Markov chains we thought they were.
    Kershenbaum A; Bowles AE; Freeberg TM; Jin DZ; Lameira AR; Bohn K
    Proc Biol Sci; 2014 Oct; 281(1792):. PubMed ID: 25143037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. A simple explanation for the evolution of complex song syntax in Bengalese finches.
    Katahira K; Suzuki K; Kagawa H; Okanoya K
    Biol Lett; 2013; 9(6):20130842. PubMed ID: 24284561
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: a parid perspective.
    Krams I; Krama T; Freeberg TM; Kullberg C; Lucas JR
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2012 Jul; 367(1597):1879-91. PubMed ID: 22641826
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants.
    Lipkind D; Marcus GF; Bemis DK; Sasahara K; Jacoby N; Takahasi M; Suzuki K; Feher O; Ravbar P; Okanoya K; Tchernichovski O
    Nature; 2013 Jun; 498(7452):104-8. PubMed ID: 23719373
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. A compact statistical model of the song syntax in Bengalese finch.
    Jin DZ; Kozhevnikov AA
    PLoS Comput Biol; 2011 Mar; 7(3):e1001108. PubMed ID: 21445230
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Social complexity can drive vocal complexity: group size influences vocal information in Carolina chickadees.
    Freeberg TM
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Jul; 17(7):557-61. PubMed ID: 16866738
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Comparative analysis of mineralocorticoid receptor expression among vocal learners (Bengalese finch and budgerigar) and non-vocal learners (quail and ring dove) has implications for the evolution of avian vocal learning.
    Matsunaga E; Suzuki K; Kobayashi T; Okanoya K
    Dev Growth Differ; 2011 Dec; 53(9):961-70. PubMed ID: 22010640
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The syntax-semantics interface in animal vocal communication.
    Suzuki TN; Wheatcroft D; Griesser M
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2020 Jan; 375(1789):20180405. PubMed ID: 31735156
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Whistle sequences in wild killer whales (Orcinus orca).
    Riesch R; Ford JK; Thomsen F
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2008 Sep; 124(3):1822-9. PubMed ID: 19045672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Syntactic structure and geographical dialects in the songs of male rock hyraxes.
    Kershenbaum A; Ilany A; Blaustein L; Geffen E
    Proc Biol Sci; 2012 Aug; 279(1740):2974-81. PubMed ID: 22513862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Vocal generalization depends on gesture identity and sequence.
    Hoffmann LA; Sober SJ
    J Neurosci; 2014 Apr; 34(16):5564-74. PubMed ID: 24741046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Recursive self-embedded vocal motifs in wild orangutans.
    Lameira AR; Hardus ME; Ravignani A; Raimondi T; Gamba M
    Elife; 2024 Jan; 12():. PubMed ID: 38252123
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Effects of domestication on neophobia: A comparison between the domesticated Bengalese finch (Lonchura striata var. domestica) and its wild ancestor, the white-rumped munia (Lonchura striata).
    Suzuki K; Ikebuchi M; Kagawa H; Koike T; Okanoya K
    Behav Processes; 2021 Dec; 193():104502. PubMed ID: 34530107
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Killer whales are capable of vocal learning.
    Foote AD; Griffin RM; Howitt D; Larsson L; Miller PJ; Hoelzel AR
    Biol Lett; 2006 Dec; 2(4):509-12. PubMed ID: 17148275
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Call combination patterns in Icelandic killer whales (Orcinus orca).
    Selbmann A; Miller PJO; Wensveen PJ; Svavarsson J; Samarra FIP
    Sci Rep; 2023 Dec; 13(1):21771. PubMed ID: 38065973
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Intra- and intergroup vocal behavior in resident killer whales, Orcinus orca.
    Weiss BM; Symonds H; Spong P; Ladich F
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2007 Dec; 122(6):3710-6. PubMed ID: 18247778
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Song motor control organizes acoustic patterns on two levels in Bengalese finches (Lonchura striata var. domestica).
    Seki Y; Suzuki K; Takahasi M; Okanoya K
    J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol; 2008 Jun; 194(6):533-43. PubMed ID: 18386017
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. [Structure, occurrence and functional significance of nonlinear phenomena in sounds of terrestrial mammals].
    Volodin IA; Volodina EV; Filatova OA
    Zh Obshch Biol; 2005; 66(4):346-62. PubMed ID: 16212284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Songbirds can learn flexible contextual control over syllable sequencing.
    Veit L; Tian LY; Monroy Hernandez CJ; Brainard MS
    Elife; 2021 Jun; 10():. PubMed ID: 34060473
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Complex sequencing rules of birdsong can be explained by simple hidden Markov processes.
    Katahira K; Suzuki K; Okanoya K; Okada M
    PLoS One; 2011; 6(9):e24516. PubMed ID: 21915345
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.