These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

219 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25202252)

  • 1. Integration of egocentric and allocentric information during memory-guided reaching to images of a natural environment.
    Fiehler K; Wolf C; Klinghammer M; Blohm G
    Front Hum Neurosci; 2014; 8():636. PubMed ID: 25202252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Scene Configuration and Object Reliability Affect the Use of Allocentric Information for Memory-Guided Reaching.
    Klinghammer M; Blohm G; Fiehler K
    Front Neurosci; 2017; 11():204. PubMed ID: 28450826
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Allocentric information is used for memory-guided reaching in depth: A virtual reality study.
    Klinghammer M; Schütz I; Blohm G; Fiehler K
    Vision Res; 2016 Dec; 129():13-24. PubMed ID: 27789230
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Contextual factors determine the use of allocentric information for reaching in a naturalistic scene.
    Klinghammer M; Blohm G; Fiehler K
    J Vis; 2015; 15(13):24. PubMed ID: 26418592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Spatial updating of allocentric landmark information in real-time and memory-guided reaching.
    Lu Z; Fiehler K
    Cortex; 2020 Apr; 125():203-214. PubMed ID: 32006875
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Egocentric cues influence the allocentric spatial memory of object configurations for memory-guided actions.
    Forster PP; Fiehler K; Karimpur H
    J Neurophysiol; 2023 Nov; 130(5):1142-1149. PubMed ID: 37791381
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The role of gaze and prior knowledge on allocentric coding of reach targets.
    Lu Z; Klinghammer M; Fiehler K
    J Vis; 2018 Apr; 18(4):22. PubMed ID: 29710312
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Further Evidence That People Rely on Egocentric Information to Guide a Cursor to a Visible Target.
    Crowe EM; Bossard M; Karimpur H; Rushton SK; Fiehler K; Brenner E
    Perception; 2021 Oct; 50(10):904-907. PubMed ID: 34617834
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Scene semantics affects allocentric spatial coding for action in naturalistic (virtual) environments.
    Baltaretu BR; Schuetz I; Võ ML; Fiehler K
    Sci Rep; 2024 Jul; 14(1):15549. PubMed ID: 38969745
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Cue reliability and a landmark stability heuristic determine relative weighting between egocentric and allocentric visual information in memory-guided reach.
    Byrne PA; Crawford JD
    J Neurophysiol; 2010 Jun; 103(6):3054-69. PubMed ID: 20457858
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Interaction between allocentric and egocentric reference frames in deaf and hearing populations.
    Zhang M; Tan X; Shen L; Wang A; Geng S; Chen Q
    Neuropsychologia; 2014 Feb; 54():68-76. PubMed ID: 24361477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The role of perception and action on the use of allocentric information in a large-scale virtual environment.
    Karimpur H; Kurz J; Fiehler K
    Exp Brain Res; 2020 Sep; 238(9):1813-1826. PubMed ID: 32500297
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Facilitation of allocentric coding by virtue of object-semantics.
    Karimpur H; Morgenstern Y; Fiehler K
    Sci Rep; 2019 Apr; 9(1):6263. PubMed ID: 31000759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Comparing the effect of temporal delay on the availability of egocentric and allocentric information in visual search.
    Ball K; Birch Y; Lane A; Ellison A; Schenk T
    Behav Brain Res; 2017 Jul; 331():38-46. PubMed ID: 28526516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Remembering object position in the absence of vision: egocentric, allocentric, and egocentric decentred frames of reference.
    Coluccia E; Mammarella IC; De Beni R; Ittyerah M; Cornoldi C
    Perception; 2007; 36(6):850-64. PubMed ID: 17718364
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. How coordinate and categorical spatial relations combine with egocentric and allocentric reference frames in a motor task: effects of delay and stimuli characteristics.
    Ruotolo F; van der Ham I; Postma A; Ruggiero G; Iachini T
    Behav Brain Res; 2015 May; 284():167-78. PubMed ID: 25698602
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Frames of reference and categorical/coordinate spatial relations in a "what was where" task.
    Ruotolo F; Iachini T; Ruggiero G; van der Ham IJ; Postma A
    Exp Brain Res; 2016 Sep; 234(9):2687-96. PubMed ID: 27180248
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Egocentric metric representations in peripersonal space: A bridge between motor resources and spatial memory.
    Ruggiero G; Ruotolo F; Orti R; Rauso B; Iachini T
    Br J Psychol; 2021 May; 112(2):433-454. PubMed ID: 32710656
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. No effect of delay on the spatial representation of serial reach targets.
    Schütz I; Henriques DY; Fiehler K
    Exp Brain Res; 2015 Apr; 233(4):1225-35. PubMed ID: 25600817
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Spatial task context makes short-latency reaches prone to induced Roelofs illusion.
    Taghizadeh B; Gail A
    Front Hum Neurosci; 2014; 8():673. PubMed ID: 25221500
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.