These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

429 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25264702)

  • 21. Cumulative risk disparities in children's neurocognitive functioning: a developmental cascade model.
    Wade M; Browne DT; Plamondon A; Daniel E; Jenkins JM
    Dev Sci; 2016 Mar; 19(2):179-94. PubMed ID: 25845409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Normal birth weight variation and children's neuropsychological functioning: links between language, executive functioning, and theory of mind.
    Wade M; Browne DT; Madigan S; Plamondon A; Jenkins JM
    J Int Neuropsychol Soc; 2014 Oct; 20(9):909-19. PubMed ID: 25171131
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Development of social-cognitive and communication skills in children born deaf.
    Peterson CC
    Scand J Psychol; 2009 Oct; 50(5):475-83. PubMed ID: 19778395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Measuring psychological outcomes following pediatric intensive care unit hospitalization: psychometric analysis of the Children's Critical Illness Impact Scale.
    Rennick JE; Johnston CC; Lambert SD; Rashotte JM; Schmitz N; Earle RJ; Stevens BJ; Tewfik T; Wood-Dauphinee S
    Pediatr Crit Care Med; 2011 Nov; 12(6):635-42. PubMed ID: 21499186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. The effect of peers' theory of mind on children's own theory of mind development: A longitudinal study in middle childhood and early adolescence.
    Lecce S; Ronchi L; Devine RT
    Dev Psychol; 2024 Jul; 60(7):1269-1278. PubMed ID: 38647470
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social competence.
    Devine RT; White N; Ensor R; Hughes C
    Dev Psychol; 2016 May; 52(5):758-71. PubMed ID: 26914214
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Executive function and theory of mind: Predictive relations from ages 2 to 4.
    Hughes C; Ensor R
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Nov; 43(6):1447-59. PubMed ID: 18020823
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Same or different? Theory of mind among children with and without disabilities.
    Smogorzewska J; Szumski G; Grygiel P
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(10):e0202553. PubMed ID: 30273356
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Relations between theory of mind and executive function in middle childhood: A short-term longitudinal study.
    Lecce S; Bianco F; Devine RT; Hughes C
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Nov; 163():69-86. PubMed ID: 28753436
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Beyond false beliefs: the development and psychometric evaluation of the perceptions of children's theory of mind measure-experimental version (PCToMM-E).
    Hutchins TL; Bonazinga LA; Prelock PA; Taylor RS
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2008 Jan; 38(1):143-55. PubMed ID: 17410414
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Translating child development research into practice: Can teachers foster children's theory of mind in primary school?
    Bianco F; Lecce S
    Br J Educ Psychol; 2016 Dec; 86(4):592-605. PubMed ID: 27520073
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Executive function and theory of mind: stability and prediction from ages 2 to 3.
    Carlson SM; Mandell DJ; Williams L
    Dev Psychol; 2004 Nov; 40(6):1105-22. PubMed ID: 15535760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Gender and theory of mind in preschoolers' group effort: evidence for timing differences behind children's earliest social loafing.
    Thompson RB; Thornton B
    J Soc Psychol; 2014; 154(6):475-9. PubMed ID: 25280164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Making sense of theory of mind and paranoia: the psychometric properties and reasoning requirements of a false belief sequencing task.
    Corcoran R; Bentall RP; Rowse G; Moore R; Cummins S; Blackwood N; Howard R; Shryane NM
    Cogn Neuropsychiatry; 2011 Nov; 16(6):547-61. PubMed ID: 21838640
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Reliability and validity of advanced theory-of-mind measures in middle childhood and adolescence.
    Hayward EO; Homer BD
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2017 Sep; 35(3):454-462. PubMed ID: 28464376
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Intergenerational transmission of theory-of-mind.
    Sabbagh MA; Seamans EL
    Dev Sci; 2008 May; 11(3):354-60. PubMed ID: 18466369
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. A meta-analytic examination of maternal reminiscing style: Elaboration, gender, and children's cognitive development.
    Waters TEA; Camia C; Facompré CR; Fivush R
    Psychol Bull; 2019 Nov; 145(11):1082-1102. PubMed ID: 31621348
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Constructing the Children's Future Thinking Questionnaire: A reliable and valid measure of children's future-oriented cognition.
    Mazachowsky TR; Mahy CEV
    Dev Psychol; 2020 Apr; 56(4):756-772. PubMed ID: 31904250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The role of theory of mind and social skills in predicting children's cheating.
    O'Connor AM; Evans AD
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Mar; 179():337-347. PubMed ID: 30579247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 22.