These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

201 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25462036)

  • 1. Is height a core geometric cue for navigation? Young children's use of height in reorientation.
    Hu Q; Zhang J; Wu D; Shao Y
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Feb; 130():123-31. PubMed ID: 25462036
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Children's use of geometry for reorientation.
    Lee SA; Spelke ES
    Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):743-9. PubMed ID: 18801130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Look up: Human adults use vertical height cues in reorientation.
    Du Y; Spetch ML; Mou W
    Mem Cognit; 2016 Nov; 44(8):1277-1287. PubMed ID: 27315804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. How fish do geometry in large and in small spaces.
    Sovrano VA; Bisazza A; Vallortigara G
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Jan; 10(1):47-54. PubMed ID: 16794851
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain.
    Vallortigara G; Pagni P; Sovrano VA
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Apr; 16(3):390-400. PubMed ID: 15072675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Dissecting the geometric module: a sense linkage for metric and landmark information in animals' spatial reorientation.
    Sovrano VA; Vallortigara G
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Jul; 17(7):616-21. PubMed ID: 16866748
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Extra-Visual Systems in the Spatial Reorientation of Cavefish.
    Sovrano VA; Potrich D; Foà A; Bertolucci C
    Sci Rep; 2018 Dec; 8(1):17698. PubMed ID: 30523284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Rats' use of geometric, featural and orientation cues to locate a hidden goal.
    Batty ER; Hoban L; Spetch ML; Dickson CT
    Behav Processes; 2009 Nov; 82(3):327-34. PubMed ID: 19683037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Young children's representation of geometric relationships between locations in location coding.
    Hu Q; Zhang M; Shao Y; Feng G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Jan; 189():104703. PubMed ID: 31655228
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment.
    Chiandetti C; Vallortigara G
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):139-46. PubMed ID: 17629754
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Navigation as a source of geometric knowledge: young children's use of length, angle, distance, and direction in a reorientation task.
    Lee SA; Sovrano VA; Spelke ES
    Cognition; 2012 Apr; 123(1):144-61. PubMed ID: 22257573
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? Insights from a rodent navigation model.
    Sheynikhovich D; Chavarriaga R; Strösslin T; Arleo A; Gerstner W
    Psychol Rev; 2009 Jul; 116(3):540-66. PubMed ID: 19618986
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Why size counts: children's spatial reorientation in large and small enclosures.
    Learmonth AE; Newcombe NS; Sheridan N; Jones M
    Dev Sci; 2008 May; 11(3):414-26. PubMed ID: 18466375
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Reorientation and landmark-guided search by young children: evidence for two systems.
    Lee SA; Shusterman A; Spelke ES
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Jul; 17(7):577-82. PubMed ID: 16866742
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick ( Gallus gallus).
    Tommasi L; Polli C
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jan; 7(1):53-9. PubMed ID: 12884079
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Very young children's memory for the location of objects in a large-scale environment.
    DeLoache JS; Brown AL
    Child Dev; 1983 Aug; 54(4):888-97. PubMed ID: 6617310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Spatial reorientation by geometry in bumblebees.
    Sovrano VA; Rigosi E; Vallortigara G
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(5):e37449. PubMed ID: 22624033
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Separation of geometric and featural information in children's spatial representation: Evidence from a model selection task.
    Li W; Hu Q; Shao Y
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2022 Jan; 213():105272. PubMed ID: 34438109
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Children's use of geometric information in mapping tasks.
    Vasilyeva M; Bowers E
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2006 Dec; 95(4):255-77. PubMed ID: 16777136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Young children's use of features to reorient is more than just associative: further evidence against a modular view of spatial processing.
    Newcombe NS; Ratliff KR; Shallcross WL; Twyman AD
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):213-20. PubMed ID: 20121877
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.