These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

145 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25521714)

  • 1. Performance of young children on ''traveling salesperson'' navigation tasks presented on a touch screen.
    Miyata H; Watanabe S; Minagawa Y
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(12):e115292. PubMed ID: 25521714
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Route selection by pigeons (Columba livia) in "traveling salesperson" navigation tasks presented on an LCD screen.
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 Nov; 124(4):433-46. PubMed ID: 20836595
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Target number influences strategy use by rats (Rattus norvegicus) in the traveling salesperson problem.
    Paez K; Blaser RE
    J Comp Psychol; 2023 Nov; 137(4):238-248. PubMed ID: 38108798
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Practice makes proficient: pigeons (Columba livia) learn efficient routes on full-circuit navigational traveling salesperson problems.
    Baron DM; Ramirez AJ; Bulitko V; Madan CR; Greiner A; Hurd PL; Spetch ML
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):53-64. PubMed ID: 24966123
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Adaptive specialization for spatial memory does not improve route efficiency: Comparing the ability of Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) and pigeons (Columba livia) to solve traveling salesperson problems.
    Kelly D; Leonard K; Gibson B
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2021 Dec; 28(6):1991-2002. PubMed ID: 34159532
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. PathGame: Crowdsourcing Time-Constrained Human Solutions for the Travelling Salesperson Problem.
    Dumnić S; Dupljanin Đ; Božović V; Ćulibrk D
    Comput Intell Neurosci; 2019; 2019():2351591. PubMed ID: 31214254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Flexible route selection by pigeons (Columba livia) on a computerized multi-goal navigation task with and without an "obstacle".
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2011 Nov; 125(4):431-435. PubMed ID: 21707140
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Pigeons and people select efficient routes when solving a one-way "traveling salesperson" task.
    Gibson BM; Wasserman EA; Kamil AC
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2007 Jul; 33(3):244-61. PubMed ID: 17620024
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Contribution of the hippocampus to performance on the traveling salesperson problem in rats.
    Hales JB; Petty EA; Collins G; Blaser RE
    Behav Brain Res; 2021 May; 405():113177. PubMed ID: 33607167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Global perceptual processing in problem solving: the case of the traveling salesperson.
    Ormerod TC; Chronicle EP
    Percept Psychophys; 1999 Aug; 61(6):1227-38. PubMed ID: 10497439
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Some factors affecting performance of rats in the traveling salesman problem.
    Bellizzi C; Goldsteinholm K; Blaser RE
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Nov; 18(6):1207-19. PubMed ID: 26123082
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Childhood wayfinding experience explains sex and individual differences in adult wayfinding strategy and anxiety.
    Vieites V; Pruden SM; Reeb-Sutherland BC
    Cogn Res Princ Implic; 2020 Mar; 5(1):12. PubMed ID: 32185533
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies.
    Bullens J; Iglói K; Berthoz A; Postma A; Rondi-Reig L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):337-50. PubMed ID: 20598705
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Route selection by rats and humans in a navigational traveling salesman problem.
    Blaser RE; Ginchansky RR
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):239-50. PubMed ID: 21927849
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies in Williams syndrome and typical development.
    Broadbent HJ; Farran EK; Tolmie A
    Dev Sci; 2014 Nov; 17(6):920-34. PubMed ID: 24702907
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Feasibility of using touch screen technology for early cognitive assessment in children.
    Twomey DM; Wrigley C; Ahearne C; Murphy R; De Haan M; Marlow N; Murray DM
    Arch Dis Child; 2018 Sep; 103(9):853-858. PubMed ID: 29535111
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Developmental gender differences in children in a virtual spatial memory task.
    León I; Cimadevilla JM; Tascón L
    Neuropsychology; 2014 Jul; 28(4):485-95. PubMed ID: 24588700
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Vervet monkeys use paths consistent with context-specific spatial movement heuristics.
    Teichroeb JA
    Ecol Evol; 2015 Oct; 5(20):4706-16. PubMed ID: 26668734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Planning paths to multiple targets: memory involvement and planning heuristics in spatial problem solving.
    Wiener JM; Ehbauer NN; Mallot HA
    Psychol Res; 2009 Sep; 73(5):644-58. PubMed ID: 18998160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.