These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

145 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25598575)

  • 1. Children's Recall of Generic and Specific Labels Regarding Animals and People.
    Gülgöz S; Gelman SA
    Cogn Dev; 2015; 33():84-98. PubMed ID: 25598575
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. This cat has nine lives? Children's memory for genericity in language.
    Gelman SA; Raman L
    Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1256-68. PubMed ID: 17723049
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Generic noun phrases in mother-child conversations.
    Pappas A; Gelman SA
    J Child Lang; 1998 Feb; 25(1):19-33. PubMed ID: 9604567
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The influence of labels and facts on children's and adults' categorization.
    Johanson M; Papafragou A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Apr; 144():130-51. PubMed ID: 26735976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Preschoolers' use of morphosyntactic cues to identify generic sentences: indefinite singular noun phrases, tense, and aspect.
    Cimpian A; Meltzer TJ; Markman EM
    Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1561-78. PubMed ID: 21790538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Children with specific language impairment: an investigation of their narratives and memory.
    Dodwell K; Bavin EL
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2008; 43(2):201-18. PubMed ID: 17852521
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Effects of generic language on category content and structure.
    Gelman SA; Ware EA; Kleinberg F
    Cogn Psychol; 2010 Nov; 61(3):273-301. PubMed ID: 20638053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Children's referential communication skills: The role of cognitive abilities and adult models of speech.
    Uzundag BA; Küntay AC
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Aug; 172():73-95. PubMed ID: 29602030
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Information learned from generic language becomes central to children's biological concepts: evidence from their open-ended explanations.
    Cimpian A; Markman EM
    Cognition; 2009 Oct; 113(1):14-25. PubMed ID: 19674739
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. A cross-linguistic comparison of generic noun phrases in English and Mandarin.
    Gelman SA; Tardif T
    Cognition; 1998 Jun; 66(3):215-48. PubMed ID: 9689770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. How we categorize objects is related to how we remember them: The shape bias as a memory bias.
    Vlach HA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Dec; 152():12-30. PubMed ID: 27454236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Memory for generic and quantified sentences in Spanish-speaking children and adults.
    Gelman SA; Sánchez Tapia I; Leslie SJ
    J Child Lang; 2016 Nov; 43(6):1231-44. PubMed ID: 26333824
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Generic Language Use Reveals Domain Differences in Children's Expectations about Animal and Artifact Categories.
    Brandone AC; Gelman SA
    Cogn Dev; 2013 Jan; 28(1):63-75. PubMed ID: 23335836
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Generic language facilitates children's cross-classification.
    Nguyen SP; Gelman SA
    Cogn Dev; 2012 Apr; 27(2):154-167. PubMed ID: 22888182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The role of recalling previous errors in middle-school children's learning.
    Loehr AM; Fazio LK; Rittle-Johnson B
    Br J Educ Psychol; 2020 Dec; 90(4):997-1014. PubMed ID: 31999838
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Factors affecting children's direct learning and productive memory processes in the context of virtual museums.
    Cronin-Golomb LM; Pejic J; Miller-Goldwater HE; Bauer PJ
    Cogn Dev; 2024; 71():. PubMed ID: 39071037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Picture versus question elaboration on young children's learning of sentences containing high- and low-probability content.
    Miller GE; Pressley M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1989 Dec; 48(3):431-50. PubMed ID: 2584923
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Setting boundaries: Development of neural and behavioral event cognition in early childhood.
    Benear SL; Popal HS; Zheng Y; Tanrıverdi B; Murty VP; Perlman SB; Olson IR; Newcombe NS
    Dev Sci; 2023 Nov; 26(6):e13409. PubMed ID: 37183213
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Beyond the basics: preschool children label objects flexibly at multiple hierarchical levels.
    Waxman SR; Hatch T
    J Child Lang; 1992 Feb; 19(1):153-66. PubMed ID: 1551929
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Counterstereotyping can change children's thinking about boys' and girls' toy preferences.
    King RA; Scott KE; Renno MP; Shutts K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Mar; 191():104753. PubMed ID: 31841820
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.