These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

189 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25678395)

  • 21. Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference.
    Grassmann S; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):252-63. PubMed ID: 20121881
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. When do infants begin to follow a point?
    Bertenthal BI; Boyer TW; Harding S
    Dev Psychol; 2014 Aug; 50(8):2036-48. PubMed ID: 24911570
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks.
    Lazarowski L; Dorman DC
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():60-7. PubMed ID: 25245303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Dingoes (Canis dingo) can use human social cues to locate hidden food.
    Smith BP; Litchfield CA
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Mar; 13(2):367-76. PubMed ID: 19779743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Reassessing shelter dogs' use of human communicative cues in the standard object-choice task.
    Osborne T; Mulcahy NJ
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213166. PubMed ID: 30845205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. What's the point? Golden and Labrador retrievers living in kennels do not understand human pointing gestures.
    D'Aniello B; Alterisio A; Scandurra A; Petremolo E; Iommelli MR; Aria M
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Jul; 20(4):777-787. PubMed ID: 28508126
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Dogs (Canis familiaris) account for body orientation but not visual barriers when responding to pointing gestures.
    MacLean EL; Krupenye C; Hare B
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Aug; 128(3):285-97. PubMed ID: 24611643
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible.
    Dwyer C; Cole MR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):442-448. PubMed ID: 30251105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. A gray seal's (Halichoerus grypus) responses to experimenter-given pointing and directional cues.
    Shapiro AD; Janik VM; Slater PJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2003 Dec; 117(4):355-62. PubMed ID: 14717636
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans.
    Miklósi A; Pongrácz P; Lakatos G; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 May; 119(2):179-86. PubMed ID: 15982161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Free-ranging dogs show age related plasticity in their ability to follow human pointing.
    Bhattacharjee D; N ND; Gupta S; Sau S; Sarkar R; Biswas A; Banerjee A; Babu D; Mehta D; Bhadra A
    PLoS One; 2017; 12(7):e0180643. PubMed ID: 28715475
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Comprehension of the communicative intent behind pointing and gazing gestures by young children with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome.
    John AE; Mervis CB
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2010 Aug; 53(4):950-60. PubMed ID: 20605941
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. When to Take a Gesture Seriously: On How We Use and Prioritize Communicative Cues.
    Gunter TC; Weinbrenner JED
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2017 Aug; 29(8):1355-1367. PubMed ID: 28358659
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. You must see the point: automatic processing of cues to the direction of social attention.
    Langton SR; Bruce V
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2000 Apr; 26(2):747-57. PubMed ID: 10811173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Free-Ranging Dogs Are Capable of Utilizing Complex Human Pointing Cues.
    Bhattacharjee D; Mandal S; Shit P; Varghese MG; Vishnoi A; Bhadra A
    Front Psychol; 2019; 10():2818. PubMed ID: 32010006
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus).
    Scheumann M; Call J
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Oct; 7(4):224-30. PubMed ID: 15057598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. The effect of development and individual differences in pointing comprehension of dogs.
    Gácsi M; Kara E; Belényi B; Topál J; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2009 May; 12(3):471-9. PubMed ID: 19130102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) comprehend the referential character of the human pointing gesture.
    Herman LM; Abichandani SL; Elhajj AN; Herman EY; Sanchez JL; Pack AA
    J Comp Psychol; 1999 Dec; 113(4):347-64. PubMed ID: 10608559
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Can dogs use vocal intonation as a social referencing cue in an object choice task?
    Colbert-White EN; Tullis A; Andresen DR; Parker KM; Patterson KE
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Mar; 21(2):253-265. PubMed ID: 29442250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.