These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
299 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25682016)
41. Beyond Simulation-Theory and Theory-Theory: why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study "theory of mind". Apperly IA Cognition; 2008 Apr; 107(1):266-83. PubMed ID: 17868666 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
42. Second-person neuroscience: implications for Wittgensteinian and Vygotskyan approaches to psychology. Moore K Behav Brain Sci; 2013 Aug; 36(4):431-2. PubMed ID: 23883760 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
43. A broad assessment of theory of mind in adolescence: the complexity of mindreading. Bosco FM; Gabbatore I; Tirassa M Conscious Cogn; 2014 Feb; 24():84-97. PubMed ID: 24491434 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
44. Adapting to the human world: dogs' responsiveness to our social cues. Reid PJ Behav Processes; 2009 Mar; 80(3):325-33. PubMed ID: 19056474 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
45. Social understanding through direct perception? Yes, by interacting. De Jaegher H Conscious Cogn; 2009 Jun; 18(2):535-42; discussion 543-50. PubMed ID: 19091603 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
46. Flexible goal attribution in early mindreading. Michael J; Christensen W Psychol Rev; 2016 Mar; 123(2):219-27. PubMed ID: 26413769 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
47. Parameterising ecological validity and integrating individual differences within second-person neuroscience. Chakrabarti B Behav Brain Sci; 2013 Aug; 36(4):414-5. PubMed ID: 23883743 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
48. More questions for mirror neurons. Borg E Conscious Cogn; 2013 Sep; 22(3):1122-31. PubMed ID: 23261329 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
49. Rethinking the ontogeny of mindreading. Tirassa M; Bosco FM; Colle L Conscious Cogn; 2006 Mar; 15(1):197-217. PubMed ID: 16099174 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
50. On projecting grammatical persons into social neurocognition: a view from linguistics. Evans N Behav Brain Sci; 2013 Aug; 36(4):419-20. PubMed ID: 23883748 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
51. Social cognition in the we-mode. Gallotti M; Frith CD Trends Cogn Sci; 2013 Apr; 17(4):160-5. PubMed ID: 23499335 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
52. 'Theory of mind' II: Difficulties and critiques. Plastow M Australas Psychiatry; 2012 Aug; 20(4):291-4. PubMed ID: 22767933 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
53. Exploring the relationship between anthropomorphism and theory-of-mind in brain and behaviour. Hortensius R; Kent M; Darda KM; Jastrzab L; Koldewyn K; Ramsey R; Cross ES Hum Brain Mapp; 2021 Sep; 42(13):4224-4241. PubMed ID: 34196439 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
54. Reaction time profiles of adults' action prediction reveal two mindreading systems. Edwards K; Low J Cognition; 2017 Mar; 160():1-16. PubMed ID: 28024170 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
55. Cultural effects on mindreading. Perez-Zapata D; Slaughter V; Henry JD Cognition; 2016 Jan; 146():410-4. PubMed ID: 26529195 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
56. Continuity in social cognition from infancy to childhood. Yamaguchi M; Kuhlmeier VA; Wynn K; vanMarle K Dev Sci; 2009 Sep; 12(5):746-52. PubMed ID: 19702767 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
57. Homo heuristicus: why biased minds make better inferences. Gigerenzer G; Brighton H Top Cogn Sci; 2009 Jan; 1(1):107-43. PubMed ID: 25164802 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
58. Further steps toward a second-person neuroscience. Sameen N; Thompson J; Carpendale JI Behav Brain Sci; 2013 Aug; 36(4):437. PubMed ID: 23883766 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
59. Pragmatic development explains the Theory-of-Mind Scale. Westra E; Carruthers P Cognition; 2017 Jan; 158():165-176. PubMed ID: 27835787 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]