These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

184 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25865749)

  • 41. Linguistic and non-linguistic non-adjacent dependency learning in early development.
    van der Kant A; Männel C; Paul M; Friederici AD; Höhle B; Wartenburger I
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2020 Oct; 45():100819. PubMed ID: 32828032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. [Phonological acquisition in preterm infants].
    Peña M; Pittaluga E; Farkas C
    Rev Neurol; 2010 Jan 1-15; 50(1):12-8. PubMed ID: 20073018
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Hippocampal and auditory contributions to speech segmentation.
    Ramos-Escobar N; Mercier M; Trébuchon-Fonséca A; Rodriguez-Fornells A; François C; Schön D
    Cortex; 2022 May; 150():1-11. PubMed ID: 35305505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. The longevity of statistical learning: When infant memory decays, isolated words come to the rescue.
    Karaman F; Hay JF
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Feb; 44(2):221-232. PubMed ID: 28782968
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Statistical learning of speech regularities can occur outside the focus of attention.
    Batterink LJ; Paller KA
    Cortex; 2019 Jun; 115():56-71. PubMed ID: 30771622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Rapid recognition at 10 months as a predictor of language development.
    Junge C; Kooijman V; Hagoort P; Cutler A
    Dev Sci; 2012 Jul; 15(4):463-73. PubMed ID: 22709396
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Rapid learning of syllable classes from a perceptually continuous speech stream.
    Endress AD; Bonatti LL
    Cognition; 2007 Nov; 105(2):247-99. PubMed ID: 17083927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Linguistic constraints on statistical computations: the role of consonants and vowels in continuous speech processing.
    Bonatti LL; Peña M; Nespor M; Mehler J
    Psychol Sci; 2005 Jun; 16(6):451-9. PubMed ID: 15943671
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Infants' sensitivity to non-adjacent dependencies across phonological phrase boundaries.
    van Heugten M; Shi R
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2010 Nov; 128(5):EL223-8. PubMed ID: 21110530
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. More efficient formation of longer-term representations for word forms at birth can be linked to better language skills at 2 years.
    Suppanen E; Winkler I; Kujala T; Ylinen S
    Dev Cogn Neurosci; 2022 Jun; 55():101113. PubMed ID: 35605476
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Longitudinal study of preterm and full-term infants: High-density EEG analyses of cortical activity in response to visual motion.
    Agyei SB; van der Weel FR; van der Meer AL
    Neuropsychologia; 2016 Apr; 84():89-104. PubMed ID: 26852826
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Brain potentials to native and non-native speech contrasts in 7- and 11-month-old American infants.
    Rivera-Gaxiola M; Silva-Pereyra J; Kuhl PK
    Dev Sci; 2005 Mar; 8(2):162-72. PubMed ID: 15720374
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Infant Diet-Related Changes in Syllable Processing Between 4 and 5 Months: Implications for Developing Native Language Sensitivity.
    Pivik RT; Andres A; Bai S; Cleves MA; Tennal KB; Gu Y; Badger TM
    Dev Neuropsychol; 2016; 41(4):215-230. PubMed ID: 27759424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Words in a sea of sounds: the output of infant statistical learning.
    Saffran JR
    Cognition; 2001 Sep; 81(2):149-69. PubMed ID: 11376640
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Headstart for speech segmentation: a neural signature for the anchor word effect.
    Cunillera T; Laine M; Rodríguez-Fornells A
    Neuropsychologia; 2016 Feb; 82():189-199. PubMed ID: 26792366
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Experience-dependent effects of passive auditory exposure in infants impact theta phase synchrony and predict later language.
    Ortiz-Mantilla S; Roesler CP; Realpe-Bonilla T; Benasich AA
    Cereb Cortex; 2023 Jun; 33(12):7595-7607. PubMed ID: 36967114
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Nine-month-olds use frequency of onset clusters to segment novel words.
    Archer SL; Curtin S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Aug; 148():131-41. PubMed ID: 27181298
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. ERP responses to lexical-semantic processing in typically developing toddlers, in adults, and in toddlers at risk for language and learning impairment.
    Cantiani C; Riva V; Piazza C; Melesi G; Mornati G; Bettoni R; Marino C; Molteni M
    Neuropsychologia; 2017 Aug; 103():115-130. PubMed ID: 28669897
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Infants' learning of novel words in a stochastic environment.
    Vouloumanos A; Werker JF
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1611-7. PubMed ID: 19899918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Tracking transitional probabilities and segmenting auditory sequences are dissociable processes in adults and neonates.
    Benjamin L; Fló A; Palu M; Naik S; Melloni L; Dehaene-Lambertz G
    Dev Sci; 2023 Mar; 26(2):e13300. PubMed ID: 35772033
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.