These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

160 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 25870573)

  • 1. She called that thing a mido, but should you call it a mido too? Linguistic experience influences infants' expectations of conventionality.
    Henderson AM; Scott JC
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():332. PubMed ID: 25870573
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Surmounting the Tower of Babel: Monolingual and bilingual 2-year-olds' understanding of the nature of foreign language words.
    Byers-Heinlein K; Chen KH; Xu F
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Mar; 119():87-100. PubMed ID: 24268905
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Who spoke that language? Assessing early face-language associations in monolingual and bilingual infants.
    Marcet L; Birulés J; Bosch L; Pons F
    Front Psychol; 2024; 15():1393836. PubMed ID: 38813567
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Language Experience Is Associated with Infants' Visual Attention to Speakers.
    Atagi N; Johnson SP
    Brain Sci; 2020 Aug; 10(8):. PubMed ID: 32823522
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Language background shapes third-party communication expectations in 14-month-old infants.
    Colomer M; Sebastian-Galles N
    Cognition; 2020 Sep; 202():104292. PubMed ID: 32460037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Language matters: thirteen-month-olds understand that the language a speaker uses constrains conventionality.
    Scott JC; Henderson AM
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Nov; 49(11):2102-11. PubMed ID: 23458661
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Who can communicate with whom? Language experience affects infants' evaluation of others as monolingual or multilingual.
    Pitts CE; Onishi KH; Vouloumanos A
    Cognition; 2015 Jan; 134():185-92. PubMed ID: 25460391
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. One language or two? Navigating cross-language conflict in statistical word segmentation.
    Antovich DM; Graf Estes K
    Dev Sci; 2020 Nov; 23(6):e12960. PubMed ID: 32145042
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Bilingualism affects 9-month-old infants' expectations about how words refer to kinds.
    Byers-Heinlein K
    Dev Sci; 2017 Jan; 20(1):. PubMed ID: 27860099
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The first steps in word learning are easier when the shoes fit: comparing monolingual and bilingual infants.
    Mattock K; Polka L; Rvachew S; Krehm M
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):229-43. PubMed ID: 20121879
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Monolingual and bilingual infants' word segmentation abilities in an inter-mixed dual-language task.
    Orena AJ; Polka L
    Infancy; 2019 Sep; 24(5):718-737. PubMed ID: 32677280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Appreciating language conventions: thirteen-month-old Chinese infants understand that word generalization is shared practice.
    Liu S; Sun R
    J Child Lang; 2019 Jul; 46(4):812-823. PubMed ID: 31018878
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Developmental change in tone perception in Mandarin monolingual, English monolingual, and Mandarin-English bilingual infants: Divergences between monolingual and bilingual learners.
    Singh L; Fu CSL; Seet XH; Tong APY; Wang JL; Best CT
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Sep; 173():59-77. PubMed ID: 29677553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Infant sensitivity to speaker and language in learning a second label.
    Bhagwat J; Casasola M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Feb; 118():41-56. PubMed ID: 24238764
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Monolingual and Bilingual Infants' Ability to Use Non-native Tone for Word Learning Deteriorates by the Second Year After Birth.
    Liu L; Kager R
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():117. PubMed ID: 29599730
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Infants' Selectively Pay Attention to the Information They Receive from a Native Speaker of Their Language.
    Marno H; Guellai B; Vidal Y; Franzoi J; Nespor M; Mehler J
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():1150. PubMed ID: 27536263
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Learning across languages: bilingual experience supports dual language statistical word segmentation.
    Antovich DM; Graf Estes K
    Dev Sci; 2018 Mar; 21(2):. PubMed ID: 28156032
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Infants' understanding of false labeling events: the referential roles of words and the speakers who use them.
    Koenig MA; Echols CH
    Cognition; 2003 Apr; 87(3):179-208. PubMed ID: 12684199
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. A Cross-Linguistic Study of Word-Mapping in 18- to 20-Month-Old Infants.
    Katerelos M; Poulin-Dubois D; Oshima-Takane Y
    Infancy; 2011 Sep; 16(5):508-534. PubMed ID: 32693550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Spanish input accelerates bilingual infants' segmentation of English words.
    Mateu V; Sundara M
    Cognition; 2022 Jan; 218():104936. PubMed ID: 34678682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.