These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

194 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26010197)

  • 21. Monkeys fail to reciprocate in an exchange task.
    Pelé M; Thierry B; Call J; Dufour V
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Sep; 13(5):745-51. PubMed ID: 20473699
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Transport of tools to food sites in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Cleveland A; Rocca AM; Wendt EL; Westergaard GC
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jul; 7(3):193-8. PubMed ID: 15022055
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Mark tests for mirror self-recognition in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) trained to touch marks.
    Roma PG; Silberberg A; Huntsberry ME; Christensen CJ; Ruggiero AM; Suomi SJ
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Sep; 69(9):989-1000. PubMed ID: 17253635
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Capuchin monkey research priorities and urgent issues.
    Lynch Alfaro JW; Izar P; Ferreira RG
    Am J Primatol; 2014 Aug; 76(8):705-20. PubMed ID: 24668460
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Do capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) know the contents of memory traces?: A study of metamemory for compound stimuli.
    Takagi S; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2018 Feb; 132(1):88-96. PubMed ID: 29283588
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Cooperative problem solving by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): spontaneous division of labor, communication, and reciprocal altruism.
    Hattori Y; Kuroshima H; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 Aug; 119(3):335-42. PubMed ID: 16131262
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Evaluation of decision-making behavior under uncertainty in capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens) using a modified Balloon Analogue Risk Task.
    Reilly OT; Brosnan SF
    J Comp Psychol; 2024 May; 138(2):88-98. PubMed ID: 38095928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use conspecifics' emotional expressions to evaluate emotional valence of objects.
    Morimoto Y; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2012 May; 15(3):341-7. PubMed ID: 21927846
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Delaying gratification for food and tokens in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): when quantity is salient, symbolic stimuli do not improve performance.
    Evans TA; Beran MJ; Paglieri F; Addessi E
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jul; 15(4):539-48. PubMed ID: 22434403
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Learning and generalization of tool use by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) in tasks involving three factors: reward, tool, and hindrance.
    Fujita K; Sato Y; Kuroshima H
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2011 Jan; 37(1):10-9. PubMed ID: 20718553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. The density bias: Capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella) prefer densely arranged items in a food-choice task.
    Parrish AE; French KA; Guild AS; Creamer CL; Rossettie MS; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2020 May; 134(2):232-240. PubMed ID: 31971398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Tool-use learning in Tonkean macaques (Macaca tonkeana).
    Ducoing AM; Thierry B
    Anim Cogn; 2005 Apr; 8(2):103-13. PubMed ID: 15449102
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Quality before quantity: rapid learning of reverse-reward contingency by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Anderson JR; Hattori Y; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 Nov; 122(4):445-8. PubMed ID: 19014269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. 'Unwilling'
    Canteloup C; Meunier H
    PeerJ; 2017; 5():e3227. PubMed ID: 28480137
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Are monkeys able to discriminate appearance from reality?
    Hirel M; Thiriau C; Roho I; Meunier H
    Cognition; 2020 Mar; 196():104123. PubMed ID: 31835097
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Summation and quantity judgments of sequentially presented sets by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Evans TA; Leighty KA; Harris EH; Rice D
    Am J Primatol; 2008 Feb; 70(2):191-4. PubMed ID: 17879377
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) show understanding of human attentional states when requesting food held by a human.
    Hattori Y; Kuroshima H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Jan; 13(1):87-92. PubMed ID: 19517147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Effect of others' attentional states on vocalizations in Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata).
    Yamaguchi C; Izumi A
    Behav Processes; 2006 Nov; 73(3):285-9. PubMed ID: 16926072
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Individual differences in co-representation in three monkey species (Callithrix jacchus, Sapajus apella and Macaca tonkeana) in the joint Simon task: the role of social factors and inhibitory control.
    Miss FM; Sadoughi B; Meunier H; Burkart JM
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Dec; 25(6):1399-1415. PubMed ID: 35508572
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Capuchin monkeys' use of human and conspecific cues to solve a hidden object-choice task.
    Essler JL; Schwartz LP; Rossettie MS; Judge PG
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Sep; 20(5):985-998. PubMed ID: 28741081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.