These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

230 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26072667)

  • 1. Child-directed teaching and social learning at 18 months of age: evidence from Yucatec Mayan and US infants.
    Shneidman L; Gaskins S; Woodward A
    Dev Sci; 2016 May; 19(3):372-81. PubMed ID: 26072667
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: how important is directed speech?
    Shneidman LA; Goldin-Meadow S
    Dev Sci; 2012 Sep; 15(5):659-73. PubMed ID: 22925514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Mayan Children's Creation of Learning Ecologies by Initiative and Cooperative Action.
    de León L
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2015; 49():153-84. PubMed ID: 26955927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Learning From Others and Spontaneous Exploration: A Cross-Cultural Investigation.
    Shneidman L; Gweon H; Schulz LE; Woodward AL
    Child Dev; 2016 May; 87(3):723-35. PubMed ID: 27189400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Deferred imitation in 18-month-olds from two cultural contexts: the case of Cameroonian Nso farmer and German-middle class infants.
    Borchert S; Lamm B; Graf F; Knopf M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2013 Dec; 36(4):717-27. PubMed ID: 24035993
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age.
    Carpenter M; Nagell K; Tomasello M
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1998; 63(4):i-vi, 1-143. PubMed ID: 9835078
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Neighborhood linguistic diversity predicts infants' social learning.
    Howard LH; Carrazza C; Woodward AL
    Cognition; 2014 Nov; 133(2):474-9. PubMed ID: 25156630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The early social significance of shared ritual actions.
    Liberman Z; Kinzler KD; Woodward AL
    Cognition; 2018 Feb; 171():42-51. PubMed ID: 29107887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Why do child-directed interactions support imitative learning in young children?
    Shneidman L; Todd R; Woodward A
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(10):e110891. PubMed ID: 25333623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The role of objects and effects in action imitation: Comparing the imitation of object-related actions vs. gestures in 18-month-old infants.
    Kim Z; Óturai G; Király I; Knopf M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2015 Nov; 41():43-51. PubMed ID: 26311467
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Human infants' understanding of social imitation: Inferences of affiliation from third party observations.
    Powell LJ; Spelke ES
    Cognition; 2018 Jan; 170():31-48. PubMed ID: 28938173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. A cross-cultural comparison of the development of the social smile: a longitudinal study of maternal and infant imitation in 6- and 12-week-old infants.
    Wörmann V; Holodynski M; Kärtner J; Keller H
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Jun; 35(3):335-47. PubMed ID: 22721734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months.
    Hilbrink EE; Sakkalou E; Ellis-Davies K; Fowler NC; Gattis M
    Dev Sci; 2013 Nov; 16(6):828-40. PubMed ID: 24118710
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Fourteen-month-old infants copy an action style accompanied by social-emotional cues.
    Fukuyama H; Myowa-Yamakoshi M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2013 Dec; 36(4):609-17. PubMed ID: 23872481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Instrumental and Conventional Interpretations of Behavior Are Associated With Distinct Outcomes in Early Childhood.
    Clegg JM; Legare CH
    Child Dev; 2016; 87(2):527-42. PubMed ID: 26682522
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Changing language input following market integration in a Yucatec Mayan community.
    Padilla-Iglesias C; Woodward AL; Goldin-Meadow S; Shneidman LA
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(6):e0252926. PubMed ID: 34153044
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Sixteen-month-old infants are sensitive to competence in third-party observational learning.
    Lee V; Rutherford MD
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Aug; 52():114-120. PubMed ID: 30007215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. A cross-cultural investigation of children's willingness to imitate prosocial and antisocial groups.
    Wilks M; Redshaw J; Mushin I; Nielsen M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Sep; 185():164-175. PubMed ID: 31154173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. "Shall we blick?" Novel words highlight actors' underlying intentions for 14-month-old infants.
    Chen ML; Waxman SR
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):426-31. PubMed ID: 22822935
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Adults' Orientation of Children--And Children's Initiative to Pitch In--To Everyday Adult Activities in a Tsotsil Maya Community.
    Martínez-Pérez M
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2015; 49():113-35. PubMed ID: 26955925
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 12.