These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

179 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26169659)

  • 21. Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner.
    Ostojić L; Clayton NS
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):445-59. PubMed ID: 23995845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Decoding a Neurofeedback-Modulated Cognitive Arousal State to Investigate Performance Regulation by the Yerkes-Dodson Law.
    Khazaei S; Amin MR; Faghih RT
    Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc; 2021 Nov; 2021():6551-6557. PubMed ID: 34892610
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Comparing pet and detection dogs (Canis familiaris) on two aspects of social cognition.
    Lazarowski L; Thompkins A; Krichbaum S; Waggoner LP; Deshpande G; Katz JS
    Learn Behav; 2020 Dec; 48(4):432-443. PubMed ID: 32607965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Do dogs rescue their owners from a stressful situation? A behavioral and physiological assessment.
    Carballo F; Dzik V; Freidin E; Damián JP; Casanave EB; Bentosela M
    Anim Cogn; 2020 Mar; 23(2):389-403. PubMed ID: 31907679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Using judgment bias test in pet and shelter dogs (Canis familiaris): Methodological and statistical caveats.
    Burani C; Barnard S; Wells D; Pelosi A; Valsecchi P
    PLoS One; 2020; 15(10):e0241344. PubMed ID: 33108399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. A disinhibitory circuit mechanism explains a general principle of peak performance during mid-level arousal.
    Beerendonk L; Mejías JF; Nuiten SA; de Gee JW; Fahrenfort JJ; van Gaal S
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2024 Jan; 121(5):e2312898121. PubMed ID: 38277436
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. The effect of domestication on inhibitory control: wolves and dogs compared.
    Marshall-Pescini S; Virányi Z; Range F
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(2):e0118469. PubMed ID: 25714840
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Breed differences in social cognition, inhibitory control, and spatial problem-solving ability in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris).
    Junttila S; Valros A; Mäki K; Väätäjä H; Reunanen E; Tiira K
    Sci Rep; 2022 Dec; 12(1):22529. PubMed ID: 36581704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. How does dominance rank status affect individual and social learning performance in the dog (Canis familiaris)?
    Pongrácz P; Vida V; Bánhegyi P; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):75-82. PubMed ID: 17492317
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Inverse sex effects on performance of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) in a repeated problem-solving task.
    Duranton C; Rödel HG; Bedossa T; Belkhir S
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Feb; 129(1):84-7. PubMed ID: 25181448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Guide dogs as a model for investigating the effect of life experience and training on gazing behaviour.
    Scandurra A; Prato-Previde E; Valsecchi P; Aria M; D'Aniello B
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jul; 18(4):937-44. PubMed ID: 25800170
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Assessment of emotional predisposition in dogs using PANAS (Positive and Negative Activation Scale) and associated relationships in a sample of dogs from Brazil.
    Savalli C; Albuquerque N; Vasconcellos AS; Ramos D; de Mello FT; Mills DS
    Sci Rep; 2019 Dec; 9(1):18386. PubMed ID: 31804536
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Thriving through relationships: assistance dogs' and companion dogs' perceived ability to contribute to thriving in individuals with and without a disability.
    Gravrok J; Howell T; Bendrups D; Bennett P
    Disabil Rehabil Assist Technol; 2020 Jan; 15(1):45-53. PubMed ID: 30689466
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Lateralized behavior and cardiac activity of dogs in response to human emotional vocalizations.
    Siniscalchi M; d'Ingeo S; Fornelli S; Quaranta A
    Sci Rep; 2018 Jan; 8(1):77. PubMed ID: 29311574
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Is a local sample internationally representative? Reproducibility of four cognitive tests in family dogs across testing sites and breeds.
    Szabó D; Mills DS; Range F; Virányi Z; Miklósi Á
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Nov; 20(6):1019-1033. PubMed ID: 28988352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Ontogenetic effects on gazing behaviour: a case study of kennel dogs (Labrador Retrievers) in the impossible task paradigm.
    D'Aniello B; Scandurra A
    Anim Cogn; 2016 May; 19(3):565-70. PubMed ID: 26803607
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Pet dogs' relationships vary rather individually than according to partner's species.
    Cimarelli G; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F; Virányi Z
    Sci Rep; 2019 Mar; 9(1):3437. PubMed ID: 30837560
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Testing the social dog hypothesis: are dogs also more skilled than chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks?
    Wobber V; Hare B
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):423-8. PubMed ID: 19376207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. High emotional reactivity toward an experimenter affects participation, but not performance, in cognitive tests with common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus).
    Schubiger MN; Wüstholz FL; Wunder A; Burkart JM
    Anim Cogn; 2015 May; 18(3):701-12. PubMed ID: 25636914
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Grumpy Dogs Are Smart Learners-The Association between Dog-Owner Relationship and Dogs' Performance in a Social Learning Task.
    Pongrácz P; Rieger G; Vékony K
    Animals (Basel); 2021 Mar; 11(4):. PubMed ID: 33808379
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.