These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

129 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26573240)

  • 1. Transitive inference of social dominance by human infants.
    Gazes RP; Hampton RR; Lourenco SF
    Dev Sci; 2017 Mar; 20(2):. PubMed ID: 26573240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Representation of stable social dominance relations by human infants.
    Mascaro O; Csibra G
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2012 May; 109(18):6862-7. PubMed ID: 22509020
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Human infants' learning of social structures: the case of dominance hierarchy.
    Mascaro O; Csibra G
    Psychol Sci; 2014 Jan; 25(1):250-5. PubMed ID: 24220623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The coevolution of transitive inference and memory capacity in the hawk-dove game.
    Doi K; Nakamaru M
    J Theor Biol; 2018 Nov; 456():91-107. PubMed ID: 30077734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Infants' representation of social hierarchies in absence of physical dominance.
    Bas J; Sebastian-Galles N
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(2):e0245450. PubMed ID: 33566835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. 'To the victor go the spoils': Infants expect resources to align with dominance structures.
    Enright EA; Gweon H; Sommerville JA
    Cognition; 2017 Jul; 164():8-21. PubMed ID: 28346870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The developmental origins of social hierarchy: how infants and young children mentally represent and respond to power and status.
    Thomsen L
    Curr Opin Psychol; 2020 Jun; 33():201-208. PubMed ID: 31783337
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Heuristics Facilitates the Evolution of Transitive Inference and Social Hierarchy in a Large Group.
    Doi K; Nakamaru M
    Acta Biotheor; 2023 Mar; 71(2):8. PubMed ID: 36867273
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Pinyon jays use transitive inference to predict social dominance.
    Paz-Y-Miño C G; Bond AB; Kamil AC; Balda RP
    Nature; 2004 Aug; 430(7001):778-81. PubMed ID: 15306809
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. When do infants expect hands to be connected to a person?
    Slaughter V; Heron-Delaney M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Jan; 108(1):220-7. PubMed ID: 20864118
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Effects of spatial training on transitive inference performance in humans and rhesus monkeys.
    Gazes RP; Lazareva OF; Bergene CN; Hampton RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2014 Oct; 40(4):477-89. PubMed ID: 25546105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Space and rank: infants expect agents in higher position to be socially dominant.
    Meng X; Nakawake Y; Nitta H; Hashiya K; Moriguchi Y
    Proc Biol Sci; 2019 Oct; 286(1912):20191674. PubMed ID: 31594505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Infants' use of social partnerships to predict behavior.
    Rhodes M; Hetherington C; Brink K; Wellman HM
    Dev Sci; 2015 Nov; 18(6):909-16. PubMed ID: 25441335
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Infants' use of movement synchrony to infer social affiliation in others.
    Fawcett C; Tunçgenç B
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Aug; 160():127-136. PubMed ID: 28427721
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Transitive inference in Polistes paper wasps.
    Tibbetts EA; Agudelo J; Pandit S; Riojas J
    Biol Lett; 2019 May; 15(5):20190015. PubMed ID: 31064309
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Fish can infer social rank by observation alone.
    Grosenick L; Clement TS; Fernald RD
    Nature; 2007 Jan; 445(7126):429-32. PubMed ID: 17251980
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Two ways to the top: evidence that dominance and prestige are distinct yet viable avenues to social rank and influence.
    Cheng JT; Tracy JL; Foulsham T; Kingstone A; Henrich J
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2013 Jan; 104(1):103-25. PubMed ID: 23163747
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Can transitive inference evolve in animals playing the hawk-dove game?
    Nakamaru M; Sasaki A
    J Theor Biol; 2003 Jun; 222(4):461-70. PubMed ID: 12781744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The effect of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact on infants' response to the Still Face Task from newborn to three months of age.
    Bigelow AE; Power M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Apr; 35(2):240-51. PubMed ID: 22245110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The effect of a looker's past reliability on infants' reasoning about beliefs.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Chow V
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1576-82. PubMed ID: 19899915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.