These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

124 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26573240)

  • 21. The effect of mother-infant skin-to-skin contact on infants' response to the Still Face Task from newborn to three months of age.
    Bigelow AE; Power M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Apr; 35(2):240-51. PubMed ID: 22245110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. The effect of a looker's past reliability on infants' reasoning about beliefs.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Chow V
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1576-82. PubMed ID: 19899915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Infants Choose Those Who Defer in Conflicts.
    Thomas AJ; Sarnecka BW
    Curr Biol; 2019 Jul; 29(13):2183-2189.e5. PubMed ID: 31231049
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Infants, mothers, and dyadic contributions to stability and prediction of social stress response at 6 months.
    Provenzi L; Olson KL; Montirosso R; Tronick E
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Jan; 52(1):1-8. PubMed ID: 26569560
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Perception and production of object-related grasping in 6-month-olds.
    Daum MM; Prinz W; Aschersleben G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Apr; 108(4):810-8. PubMed ID: 21092981
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Big and mighty: preverbal infants mentally represent social dominance.
    Thomsen L; Frankenhuis WE; Ingold-Smith M; Carey S
    Science; 2011 Jan; 331(6016):477-80. PubMed ID: 21273490
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Social Complexity Predicts Transitive Reasoning in Prosimian Primates.
    Maclean EL; Merritt DJ; Brannon EM
    Anim Behav; 2008 Aug; 76(2):479-486. PubMed ID: 19649139
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Physiological and behavioral parameters of infants' categorization: changes in heart rate and duration of examining across trials.
    Elsner B; Pauen S; Jeschonek S
    Dev Sci; 2006 Nov; 9(6):551-6. PubMed ID: 17059451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Beyond the distributional input? A developmental investigation of asymmetry in infants' categorization of cats and dogs.
    Furrer SD; Younger BA
    Dev Sci; 2005 Nov; 8(6):544-50. PubMed ID: 16246246
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Fish can infer relations between colour cues in a non-social learning task.
    La Loggia O; Rüfenacht A; Taborsky B
    Biol Lett; 2022 Nov; 18(11):20220321. PubMed ID: 36382372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Do you believe in magic? Infants' social looking during violations of expectations.
    Walden T; Kim G; McCoy C; Karrass J
    Dev Sci; 2007 Sep; 10(5):654-63. PubMed ID: 17683349
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Male long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) understand the target of facial threat.
    Overduin-de Vries AM; Bakker FA; Spruijt BM; Sterck EH
    Am J Primatol; 2016 Jul; 78(7):720-30. PubMed ID: 26872303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. The role of awareness and working memory in human transitive inference.
    Libben M; Titone D
    Behav Processes; 2008 Jan; 77(1):43-54. PubMed ID: 17703897
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The fluency of social hierarchy: the ease with which hierarchical relationships are seen, remembered, learned, and liked.
    Zitek EM; Tiedens LZ
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2012 Jan; 102(1):98-115. PubMed ID: 21910553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Infants use temporal regularities to chunk objects in memory.
    Kibbe MM; Feigenson L
    Cognition; 2016 Jan; 146():251-63. PubMed ID: 26484498
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. When logic fails: implicit transitive inference in humans.
    Frank MJ; Rudy JW; Levy WB; O'Reilly RC
    Mem Cognit; 2005 Jun; 33(4):742-50. PubMed ID: 16248338
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Can infants make transitive inferences?
    Mou Y; Province JM; Luo Y
    Cogn Psychol; 2014 Feb; 68():98-112. PubMed ID: 24316415
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Sensitivity to social contingencies between 1 and 3 months of age.
    Striano T; Henning A; Stahl D
    Dev Sci; 2005 Nov; 8(6):509-18. PubMed ID: 16246242
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. The Development of Infants' Sensitivity to Behavioral Intentions when Inferring Others' Social Preferences.
    Lee YE; Yun JE; Kim EY; Song HJ
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(9):e0135588. PubMed ID: 26383160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. 13-month-olds' understanding of social interactions.
    Choi YJ; Luo Y
    Psychol Sci; 2015 Mar; 26(3):274-83. PubMed ID: 25630442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.