BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

121 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26589653)

  • 1. Do infants perceive the social robot Keepon as a communicative partner?
    Peca A; Simut R; Cao HL; Vanderborght B
    Infant Behav Dev; 2016 Feb; 42():157-67. PubMed ID: 26589653
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Children-robot interaction: a pilot study in autism therapy.
    Kozima H; Nakagawa C; Yasuda Y
    Prog Brain Res; 2007; 164():385-400. PubMed ID: 17920443
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Can we talk to robots? Ten-month-old infants expected interactive humanoid robots to be talked to by persons.
    Arita A; Hiraki K; Kanda T; Ishiguro H
    Cognition; 2005 Apr; 95(3):B49-57. PubMed ID: 15788157
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Infants understand the referential nature of human gaze but not robot gaze.
    Okumura Y; Kanakogi Y; Kanda T; Ishiguro H; Itakura S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Sep; 116(1):86-95. PubMed ID: 23660178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Building language learning: Relations between infant attention and social contingency in the first year of life.
    Masek LR; Edgar EV; McMillan BTM; Todd JT; Golinkoff RM; Bahrick LE; Hirsh-Pasek K
    Infant Behav Dev; 2024 Jun; 75():101933. PubMed ID: 38507845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Different patterns of sensitivity differentially affect infant attention span.
    Miller JL; Hurdish E; Gros-Louis J
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Nov; 53():1-4. PubMed ID: 30339982
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Joint attention at 10 months of age in infant-mother dyads: contrasting free toy-play with semi-structured toy-play.
    Mateus V; Martins C; Osório A; Martins EC; Soares I
    Infant Behav Dev; 2013 Feb; 36(1):176-9. PubMed ID: 23254120
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Gaze following in human infants depends on communicative signals.
    Senju A; Csibra G
    Curr Biol; 2008 May; 18(9):668-71. PubMed ID: 18439827
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Does contingency in adults' responding influence 12-month-old infants' social referencing?
    Stenberg G
    Infant Behav Dev; 2017 Nov; 49():9-20. PubMed ID: 28686883
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Improving Collaborative Play Between Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders and Their Siblings: The Effectiveness of a Robot-Mediated Intervention Based on Lego® Therapy.
    Huskens B; Palmen A; Van der Werff M; Lourens T; Barakova E
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2015 Nov; 45(11):3746-55. PubMed ID: 25428293
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Effects of familiar contingencies on infants' vocal behavior in new communicative contexts.
    Miller JL
    Dev Psychobiol; 2014 Nov; 56(7):1518-27. PubMed ID: 25124029
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Joint engagement in infants and its relationship to their visual impairment measurements.
    Urqueta Alfaro A; Morash VS; Lei D; Orel-Bixler D
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Feb; 50():311-323. PubMed ID: 28619422
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Mimicry and play initiation in 18-month-old infants.
    Fawcett C; Liszkowski U
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Dec; 35(4):689-96. PubMed ID: 22986177
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. A systems view of mother-infant face-to-face communication.
    Beebe B; Messinger D; Bahrick LE; Margolis A; Buck KA; Chen H
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Apr; 52(4):556-71. PubMed ID: 26882118
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Sensing sociality in dogs: what may make an interactive robot social?
    Lakatos G; Janiak M; Malek L; Muszynski R; Konok V; Tchon K; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):387-97. PubMed ID: 24026802
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders Make a Fruit Salad with Probo, the Social Robot: An Interaction Study.
    Simut RE; Vanderfaeillie J; Peca A; Van de Perre G; Vanderborght B
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2016 Jan; 46(1):113-126. PubMed ID: 26304031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The understanding of congruent and incongruent referential gaze in 17-month-old infants: an eye-tracking study comparing human and robot.
    Manzi F; Ishikawa M; Di Dio C; Itakura S; Kanda T; Ishiguro H; Massaro D; Marchetti A
    Sci Rep; 2020 Jul; 10(1):11918. PubMed ID: 32681110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Contingency is not enough: Social context guides third-party attributions of intentional agency.
    Beier JS; Carey S
    Dev Psychol; 2014 Mar; 50(3):889-902. PubMed ID: 24099547
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The power of human gaze on infant learning.
    Okumura Y; Kanakogi Y; Kanda T; Ishiguro H; Itakura S
    Cognition; 2013 Aug; 128(2):127-33. PubMed ID: 23672983
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. A non-humanoid robot in the "uncanny valley": experimental analysis of the reaction to behavioral contingency in 2-3 year old children.
    Yamamoto K; Tanaka S; Kobayashi H; Kozima H; Hashiya K
    PLoS One; 2009 Sep; 4(9):e6974. PubMed ID: 19759818
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.