These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

250 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26863141)

  • 21. Puppies in the problem-solving paradigm: quick males and social females.
    Pinelli C; Scandurra A; Di Lucrezia A; Aria M; Semin GR; D'Aniello B
    Anim Cogn; 2023 Jun; 26(3):791-797. PubMed ID: 36417021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Ontogenetic effects on gazing behaviour: a case study of kennel dogs (Labrador Retrievers) in the impossible task paradigm.
    D'Aniello B; Scandurra A
    Anim Cogn; 2016 May; 19(3):565-70. PubMed ID: 26803607
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. A comparison of problem-solving and spatial orientation in the wolf (Canis lupus) and dog (Canis familiaris).
    Hiestand L
    Behav Genet; 2011 Nov; 41(6):840-57. PubMed ID: 21365204
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Oxytocin enhances the appropriate use of human social cues by the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) in an object choice task.
    Oliva JL; Rault JL; Appleton B; Lill A
    Anim Cogn; 2015 May; 18(3):767-75. PubMed ID: 25647172
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Search behavior in various breeds of adult dogs (Canis familiaris): object permanence and olfactory cues.
    Gagnon S; Doré FY
    J Comp Psychol; 1992 Mar; 106(1):58-68. PubMed ID: 1365009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Spatial generalization of imitation in dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Fugazza C; Pogány Á; Miklósi Á
    J Comp Psychol; 2016 Aug; 130(3):249-58. PubMed ID: 27124400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Dogs (Canis familiaris) can learn to attend to connectivity in string pulling tasks.
    Riemer S; Müller C; Range F; Huber L
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Feb; 128(1):31-9. PubMed ID: 23875921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. The use of a displacement device negatively affects the performance of dogs (Canis familiaris) in visible object displacement tasks.
    Müller CA; Riemer S; Range F; Huber L
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Aug; 128(3):240-50. PubMed ID: 24611641
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Why do dogs look back at the human in an impossible task? Looking back behaviour may be over-interpreted.
    Lazzaroni M; Marshall-Pescini S; Manzenreiter H; Gosch S; Přibilová L; Darc L; McGetrick J; Range F
    Anim Cogn; 2020 May; 23(3):427-441. PubMed ID: 32090291
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Increase in reaction time for solving problems during learning-set formation.
    Yokoyama C; Onoe H; Watanabe Y
    Behav Brain Res; 2004 Jul; 152(2):221-9. PubMed ID: 15196789
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Is previous experience important for inhibitory control? A comparison between shelter and pet dogs in A-not-B and cylinder tasks.
    Fagnani J; Barrera G; Carballo F; Bentosela M
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Nov; 19(6):1165-1172. PubMed ID: 27541147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Effect of Sex and Reproductive Status on Inhibitory Control and Social Cognition in the Domestic Dog (
    Junttila S; Huohvanainen S; Tiira K
    Animals (Basel); 2021 Aug; 11(8):. PubMed ID: 34438905
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Object play and problem solving in infancy: Insights into tool use.
    Rat-Fischer L; Plunkett K; von Bayern AMP; Kacelnik A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2024 Aug; 244():105957. PubMed ID: 38805863
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks.
    Lazarowski L; Dorman DC
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():60-7. PubMed ID: 25245303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Non-visual spatial tasks reveal increased interactions with stance postural control.
    Woollacott M; Vander Velde T
    Brain Res; 2008 May; 1208():95-102. PubMed ID: 18394592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Spatial memory of domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) for hidden objects in a detour task.
    Fiset S; Beaulieu C; LeBlanc V; Dubé L
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2007 Oct; 33(4):497-508. PubMed ID: 17924796
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Temperament and problem solving in a population of adolescent guide dogs.
    Bray EE; Sammel MD; Seyfarth RM; Serpell JA; Cheney DL
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Sep; 20(5):923-939. PubMed ID: 28695349
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Characterizing innovators: Ecological and individual predictors of problem-solving performance.
    Prasher S; Evans JC; Thompson MJ; Morand-Ferron J
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(6):e0217464. PubMed ID: 31188843
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Dogs learn to solve the support problem based on perceptual cues.
    Müller CA; Riemer S; Virányi Z; Huber L; Range F
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Sep; 17(5):1071-80. PubMed ID: 24590804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) coordinate their actions in a problem-solving task.
    Bräuer J; Bös M; Call J; Tomasello M
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Mar; 16(2):273-85. PubMed ID: 23090682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 13.