BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

622 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26881902)

  • 1. Suboptimal choice in pigeons: Choice is primarily based on the value of the conditioned reinforcer rather than overall reinforcement rate.
    Smith AP; Zentall TR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2016 Apr; 42(2):212-20. PubMed ID: 26881902
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Pigeons prefer discriminative stimuli independently of the overall probability of reinforcement and of the number of presentations of the conditioned reinforcer.
    Stagner JP; Laude JR; Zentall TR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2012 Oct; 38(4):446-52. PubMed ID: 23066982
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Do pigeons prefer alternatives that include near-hit outcomes?
    Stagner JP; Case JP; Sticklen MF; Duncan AK; Zentall TR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2015 Jul; 41(3):247-54. PubMed ID: 26167775
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Rats' preferences in the suboptimal choice procedure: Evaluating the impact of reinforcement probability and conditioned inhibitors.
    Alba R; Rodríguez W; Martínez M; Orduña V
    Behav Processes; 2018 Dec; 157():574-582. PubMed ID: 29689334
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Suboptimal Choice in Pigeons: Stimulus Value Predicts Choice over Frequencies.
    Smith AP; Bailey AR; Chow JJ; Beckmann JS; Zentall TR
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(7):e0159336. PubMed ID: 27441394
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Suboptimal choice in pigeons: Does the predictive value of the conditioned reinforcer alone determine choice?
    Case JP; Zentall TR
    Behav Processes; 2018 Dec; 157():320-326. PubMed ID: 30077654
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Rats are optimal in a choice task in which pigeons are not.
    Trujano RE; Orduña V
    Behav Processes; 2015 Oct; 119():22-7. PubMed ID: 26200394
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The essential value of the alternatives of the suboptimal choice procedure is different for pigeons and rats.
    López-Tolsa GE; Niño JM; Orduña V
    Behav Processes; 2020 Dec; 181():104245. PubMed ID: 32991942
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Procrastination in the pigeon: Can conditioned reinforcement increase the likelihood of human procrastination?
    Zentall TR; Case JP; Andrews DM
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2018 Oct; 25(5):1952-1957. PubMed ID: 29188447
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Optimal behavior by rats in a choice task is associated to a persistent conditioned inhibition effect.
    Trujano RE; López P; Rojas-Leguizamón M; Orduña V
    Behav Processes; 2016 Sep; 130():65-70. PubMed ID: 27421608
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Choosing among multiple alternatives: Relative and overall reinforcer rates.
    Beeby E; Alsop B
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2017 Sep; 108(2):204-222. PubMed ID: 28758210
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Contrast between what is expected and what occurs increases pigeon's suboptimal choice.
    Zentall TR; Andrews DM; Case JP
    Anim Cogn; 2019 Jan; 22(1):81-87. PubMed ID: 30430348
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Signaled reinforcement: Effects of signal reliability on choice between signaled and unsignaled alternatives.
    Gomes-Ng S; Macababbad AC; Bai JYH; Baharrizki D; Elliffe D; Cowie S
    Behav Processes; 2020 May; 174():104088. PubMed ID: 32092454
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Suboptimal choice by pigeons is eliminated when key-pecking behavior is replaced by treadle-pressing.
    González-Torres R; Flores J; Orduña V
    Behav Processes; 2020 Sep; 178():104157. PubMed ID: 32497555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Dual effects on choice of conditioned reinforcement frequency and conditioned reinforcement value.
    McDevitt MA; Williams BA
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2010 Mar; 93(2):147-55. PubMed ID: 20885807
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The role of contingency discriminability in suboptimal choice.
    López-Tolsa GE; Orduña V
    Behav Processes; 2021 Dec; 193():104511. PubMed ID: 34562512
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Choice with probabilistic reinforcement: effects of delay and conditioned reinforcers.
    Mazur JE
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1991 Jan; 55(1):63-77. PubMed ID: 2002302
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Suboptimal choice by pigeons may result from the diminishing effect of nonreinforcement.
    Laude JR; Stagner JP; Zentall TR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2014 Jan; 40(1):12-21. PubMed ID: 24893105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Choice with delayed and probabilistic reinforcers: effects of variability, time between trials, and conditioned reinforcers.
    Mazur JE; Romano A
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1992 Nov; 58(3):513-25. PubMed ID: 1447543
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Context effects on choice.
    Goldshmidt JN; Lattal KM; Fantino E
    J Exp Anal Behav; 1998 Nov; 70(3):301-20. PubMed ID: 9821681
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 32.