These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

167 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26905502)

  • 41. Development of rapid word-object associations in relation to expressive vocabulary: Shared commonalities in infants and toddlers with and without Williams syndrome.
    Ha OR; Cashon CH; Holt NA; Mervis CB
    Dev Sci; 2020 Nov; 23(6):e12966. PubMed ID: 32196857
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. When half a word is enough: infants can recognize spoken words using partial phonetic information.
    Fernald A; Swingley D; Pinto JP
    Child Dev; 2001; 72(4):1003-15. PubMed ID: 11480931
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Boosting the input: 9-month-olds' sensitivity to low-frequency phonotactic patterns in novel wordforms.
    Archer SL; Czarnecki N; Curtin S
    Infancy; 2021 Sep; 26(5):745-755. PubMed ID: 34297896
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Audiovisual speech perception in infancy: The influence of vowel identity and infants' productive abilities on sensitivity to (mis)matches between auditory and visual speech cues.
    Altvater-Mackensen N; Mani N; Grossmann T
    Dev Psychol; 2016 Feb; 52(2):191-204. PubMed ID: 26595352
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Distributional regularity and phonotactic constraints are useful for segmentation.
    Brent MR; Cartwright TA
    Cognition; 1996; 61(1-2):93-125. PubMed ID: 8990969
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Prosody and Function Words Cue the Acquisition of Word Meanings in 18-Month-Old Infants.
    de Carvalho A; He AX; Lidz J; Christophe A
    Psychol Sci; 2019 Mar; 30(3):319-332. PubMed ID: 30668928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. The effect of incremental changes in phonotactic probability and neighborhood density on word learning by preschool children.
    Storkel HL; Bontempo DE; Aschenbrenner AJ; Maekawa J; Lee SY
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2013 Oct; 56(5):1689-700. PubMed ID: 23882005
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Narrowing the distance to language: one step at a time.
    Jusczyk PW
    J Commun Disord; 1999; 32(4):207-22. PubMed ID: 10466094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. How infants begin to extract words from speech.
    Jusczyk PW
    Trends Cogn Sci; 1999 Sep; 3(9):323-328. PubMed ID: 10461194
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Inferior frontal sensitivity to common speech sounds is amplified by increasing word intelligibility.
    Vaden KI; Kuchinsky SE; Keren NI; Harris KC; Ahlstrom JB; Dubno JR; Eckert MA
    Neuropsychologia; 2011 Nov; 49(13):3563-72. PubMed ID: 21925521
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Bye-bye mummy - Word comprehension in 9-month-old infants.
    Syrnyk C; Meints K
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2017 Jun; 35(2):202-217. PubMed ID: 27621053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Lexical stress constrains English-learning infants' segmentation in a non-native language.
    Sundara M; Mateu VE
    Cognition; 2018 Dec; 181():105-116. PubMed ID: 30176405
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. The curse of knowledge: first language knowledge impairs adult learners' use of novel statistics for word segmentation.
    Finn AS; Hudson Kam CL
    Cognition; 2008 Aug; 108(2):477-99. PubMed ID: 18533142
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Cross-language sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in infants.
    Kajikawa S; Fais L; Mugitani R; Werker JF; Amano S
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2006 Oct; 120(4):2278-84. PubMed ID: 17069323
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers.
    Messer MH; Leseman PP; Boom J; Mayo AY
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Apr; 105(4):306-23. PubMed ID: 20116805
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience.
    Chambers KE; Onishi KH; Fisher C
    Cognition; 2003 Mar; 87(2):B69-77. PubMed ID: 12590043
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Pattern induction by infant language learners.
    Saffran JR; Thiessen ED
    Dev Psychol; 2003 May; 39(3):484-94. PubMed ID: 12760517
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Referential labeling can facilitate phonetic learning in infancy.
    Yeung HH; Chen LM; Werker JF
    Child Dev; 2014; 85(3):1036-49. PubMed ID: 24936610
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Using speech sounds to guide word learning: the case of bilingual infants.
    Fennell CT; Byers-Heinlein K; Werker JF
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(5):1510-25. PubMed ID: 17883445
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Understanding Bilingual Word Learning: The Role of Phonotactic Probability and Phonological Neighborhood Density.
    Nair VK; Biedermann B; Nickels L
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2017 Dec; 60(12):3551-3560. PubMed ID: 29114764
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.