These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

113 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 26941682)

  • 1. Goal Attribution toward Non-Human Objects during Infancy Predicts Imaginary Companion Status during Preschool Years.
    Moriguchi Y; Kanakogi Y; Todo N; Okumura Y; Shinohara I; Itakura S
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():221. PubMed ID: 26941682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Did Chinese children with imaginary companions attribute more agencies to non-human items: Evidences from behavioral cues and appearance characteristics.
    Qiyi L; Ruiyi Z; Yiwen Z; Nan Z
    Front Psychol; 2022; 13():899047. PubMed ID: 36160518
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Chinese children's imaginary companions: Relations with peer relationships and social competence.
    Lin Q; Fu H; Wan Y; Zhou N; Xu H
    Int J Psychol; 2018 Oct; 53(5):388-396. PubMed ID: 27665732
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Action effects foster 11-month-olds' prediction of action goals for a non-human agent.
    Adam M; Elsner B
    Infant Behav Dev; 2018 Nov; 53():49-55. PubMed ID: 30262180
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Goal saliency boosts infants' action prediction for human manual actions, but not for mechanical claws.
    Adam M; Reitenbach I; Papenmeier F; Gredebäck G; Elsner C; Elsner B
    Infant Behav Dev; 2016 Aug; 44():29-37. PubMed ID: 27267784
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Prevalence of imaginary companions in Japanese children.
    Moriguchi Y; Todo N
    Int J Psychol; 2019 Apr; 54(2):269-276. PubMed ID: 28718194
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Imaginary Companions in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder.
    Davis PE; Simon H; Meins E; Robins DL
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2018 Aug; 48(8):2790-2799. PubMed ID: 29564680
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Infants' perception of goal-directed actions: A multi-lab replication reveals that infants anticipate paths and not goals.
    Ganglmayer K; Attig M; Daum MM; Paulus M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2019 Nov; 57():101340. PubMed ID: 31387059
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Reaching the goal: Active experience facilitates 8-month-old infants' prospective analysis of goal-based actions.
    Krogh-Jespersen S; Woodward AL
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Jul; 171():31-45. PubMed ID: 29499431
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Goal prediction in 2-year-old children with and without autism spectrum disorder: An eye-tracking study.
    Krogh-Jespersen S; Kaldy Z; Valadez AG; Carter AS; Woodward AL
    Autism Res; 2018 Jun; 11(6):870-882. PubMed ID: 29405645
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants.
    Csibra G
    Cognition; 2008 May; 107(2):705-17. PubMed ID: 17869235
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Actors and actions: The role of agent behavior in infants' attribution of goals.
    Johnson SC; Shimizu YA; Ok SJ
    Cogn Dev; 2007 Jul; 22(3):310-322. PubMed ID: 18591991
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Goal certainty modulates infants' goal-directed gaze shifts.
    Henrichs I; Elsner C; Elsner B; Wilkinson N; Gredebäck G
    Dev Psychol; 2014 Jan; 50(1):100-7. PubMed ID: 23586939
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Action type and goal type modulate goal-directed gaze shifts in 14-month-old infants.
    Gredebäck G; Stasiewicz D; Falck-Ytter T; von Hofsten C; Rosander K
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Jul; 45(4):1190-4. PubMed ID: 19586188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Infants attribute goals even to biomechanically impossible actions.
    Southgate V; Johnson MH; Csibra G
    Cognition; 2008 Jun; 107(3):1059-69. PubMed ID: 18078920
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Origins of the concepts cause, cost, and goal in prereaching infants.
    Liu S; Brooks NB; Spelke ES
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2019 Sep; 116(36):17747-17752. PubMed ID: 31431537
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Twelve-month-old infants interpret action in context.
    Woodward AL; Sommerville JA
    Psychol Sci; 2000 Jan; 11(1):73-7. PubMed ID: 11228848
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Shifting goals: effects of active and observational experience on infants' understanding of higher order goals.
    Gerson SA; Mahajan N; Sommerville JA; Matz L; Woodward AL
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():310. PubMed ID: 25852622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Children with Imaginary Companions Focus on Mental Characteristics When Describing Their Real-Life Friends.
    Davis PE; Meins E; Fernyhough C
    Infant Child Dev; 2014 Nov; 23(6):622-633. PubMed ID: 25685093
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.