These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

147 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27294711)

  • 21. "Where are the . . . Fixations?": Grammatical number cues guide anticipatory fixations to upcoming referents and reduce lexical competition.
    Brown VA; Fox NP; Strand JF
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2022 May; 48(5):643-657. PubMed ID: 34014757
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Out-of-vocabulary but not meaningless: Evidence for semantic-priming effects in pseudoword processing.
    Gatti D; Marelli M; Rinaldi L
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2023 Mar; 152(3):851-863. PubMed ID: 36174173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Learning grammatical categories from distributional cues: flexible frames for language acquisition.
    St Clair MC; Monaghan P; Christiansen MH
    Cognition; 2010 Sep; 116(3):341-60. PubMed ID: 20674613
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The structure of verbal abilities in young and older adults.
    Kemper S; Sumner A
    Psychol Aging; 2001 Jun; 16(2):312-22. PubMed ID: 11405318
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Learning words and rules: abstract knowledge of word order in early sentence comprehension.
    Gertner Y; Fisher C; Eisengart J
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Aug; 17(8):684-91. PubMed ID: 16913951
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Why segmentation matters: Experience-driven segmentation errors impair "morpheme" learning.
    Finn AS; Hudson Kam CL
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2015 Sep; 41(5):1560-9. PubMed ID: 25730305
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Grammar or serial order?: discrete combinatorial brain mechanisms reflected by the syntactic mismatch negativity.
    Pulvermüller F; Assadollahi R
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2007 Jun; 19(6):971-80. PubMed ID: 17536967
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. When it hurts (and helps) to try: the role of effort in language learning.
    Finn AS; Lee T; Kraus A; Hudson Kam CL
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(7):e101806. PubMed ID: 25047901
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Similarities and differences in the lexical-grammatical relation of young dual language learners with and without specific language impairment.
    Simon-Cereijido G; Méndez LI
    Clin Linguist Phon; 2020; 34(1-2):92-109. PubMed ID: 31092018
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Effects of statistical learning on the acquisition of grammatical categories through Qur'anic memorization: A natural experiment.
    Zuhurudeen FM; Huang YT
    Cognition; 2016 Mar; 148():79-84. PubMed ID: 26744807
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Can adults learn L2 grammar after prolonged exposure under incidental conditions?
    Kenanidis P; Dąbrowska E; Llompart M; Pili-Moss D
    PLoS One; 2023; 18(7):e0288989. PubMed ID: 37494310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. The bilingual advantage in novel word learning.
    Kaushanskaya M; Marian V
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2009 Aug; 16(4):705-10. PubMed ID: 19648456
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Clinical markers in Italian-speaking children with and without specific language impairment: a study of non-word and real word repetition as predictors of grammatical ability.
    Dispaldro M; Leonard LB; Deevy P
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2013; 48(5):554-64. PubMed ID: 24033653
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners.
    Verhagen J; Leseman P
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Jan; 141():65-82. PubMed ID: 26340756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Category induction from distributional cues in an artificial language.
    Mintz TH
    Mem Cognit; 2002 Jul; 30(5):678-86. PubMed ID: 12219885
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Acquiring explicit grammatical knowledge using the Spanish verbs ser and estar as examples.
    Winitz H; Sagarna B
    J Psycholinguist Res; 2007 Jul; 36(4):319-39. PubMed ID: 17347878
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The phonological-distributional coherence hypothesis: cross-linguistic evidence in language acquisition.
    Monaghan P; Christiansen MH; Chater N
    Cogn Psychol; 2007 Dec; 55(4):259-305. PubMed ID: 17291481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Effectiveness of intervention focused on vocational course vocabulary in post-16 students with (developmental) language disorder.
    Ebbels SH; Bannister L; Holland B; Campbell L
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2022 Nov; 57(6):1334-1353. PubMed ID: 35859265
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Implicit statistical learning in language processing: word predictability is the key.
    Conway CM; Bauernschmidt A; Huang SS; Pisoni DB
    Cognition; 2010 Mar; 114(3):356-71. PubMed ID: 19922909
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Children's use of phonology to infer grammatical class in vocabulary learning.
    Cassidy KW; Kelly MH
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2001 Sep; 8(3):519-23. PubMed ID: 11700903
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.