BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

225 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27416301)

  • 1. Young children infer preferences from a single action, but not if it is constrained.
    Pesowski ML; Denison S; Friedman O
    Cognition; 2016 Oct; 155():168-175. PubMed ID: 27416301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Children's understanding of the costs and rewards underlying rational action.
    Jara-Ettinger J; Gweon H; Tenenbaum JB; Schulz LE
    Cognition; 2015 Jul; 140():14-23. PubMed ID: 25867996
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Using versus liking: Young children use ownership to predict actions but not to infer preferences.
    Pesowski ML; Friedman O
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 May; 169():19-29. PubMed ID: 29324243
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The Development of Infants' Sensitivity to Behavioral Intentions when Inferring Others' Social Preferences.
    Lee YE; Yun JE; Kim EY; Song HJ
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(9):e0135588. PubMed ID: 26383160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Children's Judgments of Epistemic and Moral Agents: From Situations to Intentions.
    Koenig MA; Tiberius V; Hamlin JK
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2019 May; 14(3):344-360. PubMed ID: 30629887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Verbal framing of statistical evidence drives children's preference inferences.
    Garvin LE; Woodward AL
    Cognition; 2015 May; 138():35-48. PubMed ID: 25704581
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Developing intuitions about free will between ages four and six.
    Kushnir T; Gopnik A; Chernyak N; Seiver E; Wellman HM
    Cognition; 2015 May; 138():79-101. PubMed ID: 25721020
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Young children use statistical sampling to infer the preferences of other people.
    Kushnir T; Xu F; Wellman HM
    Psychol Sci; 2010 Aug; 21(8):1134-40. PubMed ID: 20622142
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Young children's preference for unique owned objects.
    Gelman SA; Davidson NS
    Cognition; 2016 Oct; 155():146-154. PubMed ID: 27395441
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Young children's use of statistical sampling evidence to infer the subjectivity of preferences.
    Ma L; Xu F
    Cognition; 2011 Sep; 120(3):403-11. PubMed ID: 21353215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Give and take: Ownership affects how 2- and 3-year-olds allocate resources.
    Pesowski ML; Kanngiesser P; Friedman O
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Sep; 185():214-223. PubMed ID: 31097201
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Children's and adults' use of verbal information to visually anticipate others' actions: A study on explicit and implicit social-cognitive processing.
    Paulus M; Schuwerk T; Sodian B; Ganglmayer K
    Cognition; 2017 Mar; 160():145-152. PubMed ID: 28088714
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Social sampling: Children track social choices to reason about status hierarchies.
    Heck IA; Kushnir T; Kinzler KD
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2021 Aug; 150(8):1673-1687. PubMed ID: 33523688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The influence of a bystander agent's beliefs on children's and adults' decision-making process.
    Buttelmann F; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jan; 153():126-139. PubMed ID: 27741442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. How barriers become invisible: Children are less sensitive to constraints that are stable over time.
    Amemiya J; Heyman GD; Walker CM
    Dev Sci; 2024 Jul; 27(4):e13496. PubMed ID: 38494598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Building representations of the social world: Children extract patterns from social choices to reason about multi-group hierarchies.
    Heck IA; Kushnir T; Kinzler KD
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13366. PubMed ID: 36588167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Negativity bias in infants' expectations about agents' dispositions.
    Chae JJK; Song HJ
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2018 Nov; 36(4):620-633. PubMed ID: 29717497
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Young children integrate current observations, priors and agent information to predict others' actions.
    Kayhan E; Heil L; Kwisthout J; van Rooij I; Hunnius S; Bekkering H
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(5):e0200976. PubMed ID: 31116742
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. A comparison of American and Nepalese children's concepts of freedom of choice and social constraint.
    Chernyak N; Kushnir T; Sullivan KM; Wang Q
    Cogn Sci; 2013; 37(7):1343-55. PubMed ID: 23692272
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Early androgens, activity levels and toy choices of children in the second year of life.
    Alexander GM; Saenz J
    Horm Behav; 2012 Sep; 62(4):500-4. PubMed ID: 22955184
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 12.