These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

252 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27434675)

  • 1. Stroop-like effects in a new-code learning task: A cognitive load theory perspective.
    Hazan-Liran B; Miller P
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2017 Sep; 70(9):1878-1891. PubMed ID: 27434675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Does task-irrelevant colour information create extraneous cognitive load? Evidence from a learning task.
    Miller P; Hazan-Liran B; Cohen D
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2019 May; 72(5):1155-1163. PubMed ID: 29788826
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The Influence of Manipulating and Accentuating Task-Irrelevant Information on Learning Efficiency: Insights for Cognitive Load Theory.
    Hazan-Liran B; Miller P
    J Cogn; 2024; 7(1):36. PubMed ID: 38638463
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Attention, spatial integration, and the tail of response time distributions in Stroop task performance.
    Roelofs A
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2012; 65(1):135-50. PubMed ID: 21936655
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Dissociating stimulus-stimulus and response-response effects in the Stroop task.
    Schmidt JR; Cheesman J
    Can J Exp Psychol; 2005 Jun; 59(2):132-8. PubMed ID: 16035346
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. You can't Stroop a lexical decision: is semantic processing fundamentally facilitative?
    Schmidt JR; Cheesman J; Besner D
    Can J Exp Psychol; 2013 Jun; 67(2):130-9. PubMed ID: 23205510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Stop interfering: Stroop task conflict independence from informational conflict and interference.
    Kalanthroff E; Goldfarb L; Usher M; Henik A
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2013; 66(7):1356-67. PubMed ID: 23163896
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The semantic Stroop effect is controlled by endogenous attention.
    Kinoshita S; Mills L; Norris D
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Nov; 44(11):1730-1742. PubMed ID: 29672118
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Attention and facilitation: converging information versus inadvertent reading in Stroop task performance.
    Roelofs A
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2010 Mar; 36(2):411-22. PubMed ID: 20192539
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Transitions between color categories mapped with a reverse Stroop task.
    Smithson HE; Khan SS; Sharpe LT; Stockman A
    Vis Neurosci; 2006; 23(3-4):453-60. PubMed ID: 16961980
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Does temporal contiguity moderate contingency learning in a speeded performance task?
    Schmidt JR; De Houwer J
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2012; 65(3):408-25. PubMed ID: 22224822
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Ink colours as task-irrelevant information decrease memory accuracy with the Associative Memory Stroop Task.
    Ishikawa M; Sharma D
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2021 May; 216():103306. PubMed ID: 33866276
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Animacy and attentional processes: Evidence from the Stroop task.
    Bugaiska A; Grégoire L; Camblats AM; Gelin M; Méot A; Bonin P
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2019 Apr; 72(4):882-889. PubMed ID: 29716460
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Anterior cingulate cortex and the Stroop task: neuropsychological evidence for topographic specificity.
    Swick D; Jovanovic J
    Neuropsychologia; 2002; 40(8):1240-53. PubMed ID: 11931927
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The color-word Stroop effect driven by working memory maintenance.
    Pan Y; Han Y; Zuo W
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2019 Nov; 81(8):2722-2731. PubMed ID: 31214969
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Contingency learning and unlearning in the blink of an eye: a resource dependent process.
    Schmidt JR; Houwer JD; Besner D
    Conscious Cogn; 2010 Mar; 19(1):235-50. PubMed ID: 20116294
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Incidental learning of incongruent items in a manual version of the Stroop task.
    Lemercier C
    Percept Mot Skills; 2009 Jun; 108(3):705-16. PubMed ID: 19725307
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Functional connectivity during Stroop task performance.
    Harrison BJ; Shaw M; Yücel M; Purcell R; Brewer WJ; Strother SC; Egan GF; Olver JS; Nathan PJ; Pantelis C
    Neuroimage; 2005 Jan; 24(1):181-91. PubMed ID: 15588609
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Bliss is blue and bleak is grey: Abstract word-colour associations influence objective performance even when not task relevant.
    Goodhew SC; Kidd E
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2020 May; 206():103067. PubMed ID: 32283349
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Enduring cognitive dysfunction in unipolar major depression: a test-retest study using the Stroop paradigm.
    Hammar A; Sørensen L; Ardal G; Oedegaard KJ; Kroken R; Roness A; Lund A
    Scand J Psychol; 2010 Aug; 51(4):304-8. PubMed ID: 20042028
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 13.