BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

319 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27564429)

  • 21. Investigating the Function of Mutual Grooming in Captive Bonobos (Pan paniscus) and Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Allanic M; Hayashi M; Matsuzawa T
    Folia Primatol (Basel); 2020; 91(5):481-494. PubMed ID: 32229727
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Behavioral and functional strategies during tool use tasks in bonobos.
    Bardo A; Borel A; Meunier H; Guéry JP; Pouydebat E
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2016 Sep; 161(1):125-40. PubMed ID: 27311774
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Franco-Japanese and other collaborative contributions to understanding chimpanzee culture at Bossou and the Nimba Mountains.
    Humle T
    Primates; 2016 Jul; 57(3):339-48. PubMed ID: 27083926
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Perspectives on object manipulation and action grammar for percussive actions in primates.
    Hayashi M
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2015 Nov; 370(1682):. PubMed ID: 26483528
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Social grooming among wild bonobos (Pan paniscus) at Wamba in the Luo Scientific Reserve, DR Congo, with special reference to the formation of grooming gatherings.
    Sakamaki T
    Primates; 2013 Oct; 54(4):349-59. PubMed ID: 23625035
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Wild capuchin monkeys spontaneously adjust actions when using hammer stones of different mass to crack nuts of different resistance.
    Liu Q; Fragaszy DM; Visalberghi E
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2016 Sep; 161(1):53-61. PubMed ID: 27135182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. How similar are nut-cracking and stone-flaking? A functional approach to percussive technology.
    Bril B; Parry R; Dietrich G
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2015 Nov; 370(1682):. PubMed ID: 26483533
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. First report of a leopard (Panthera pardus)-bonobo (Pan paniscus) encounter at the LuiKotale study site, Democratic Republic of the Congo.
    Corredor-Ospina N; Kreyer M; Rossi G; Hohmann G; Fruth B
    Primates; 2021 Jul; 62(4):555-562. PubMed ID: 33950405
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Do chimpanzees anticipate an object's weight? A field experiment on the kinematics of hammer-lifting movements in the nut-cracking Taï chimpanzees.
    Sirianni G; Wittig RM; Gratton P; Mundry R; Schüler A; Boesch C
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Jan; 21(1):109-118. PubMed ID: 29196908
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Evidence for the consumption of arboreal, diurnal primates by bonobos (Pan paniscus).
    Surbeck M; Fowler A; Deimel C; Hohmann G
    Am J Primatol; 2009 Feb; 71(2):171-4. PubMed ID: 19058132
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. On the tool use behavior of the bonobo-chimpanzee last common ancestor, and the origins of hominine stone tool use.
    Haslam M
    Am J Primatol; 2014 Oct; 76(10):910-8. PubMed ID: 24710771
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Adult and juvenile bearded capuchin monkeys handle stone hammers differently during nut-cracking.
    Fragaszy DM; Barton SA; Keo S; Patel R; Izar P; Visalberghi E; Haslam M
    Am J Primatol; 2020 Jul; 82(7):e23156. PubMed ID: 32458474
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Field experiments find no evidence that chimpanzee nut cracking can be independently innovated.
    Koops K; Soumah AG; van Leeuwen KL; Camara HD; Matsuzawa T
    Nat Hum Behav; 2022 Apr; 6(4):487-494. PubMed ID: 35075258
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Revisiting Panda 100, the first archaeological chimpanzee nut-cracking site.
    Proffitt T; Haslam M; Mercader JF; Boesch C; Luncz LV
    J Hum Evol; 2018 Nov; 124():117-139. PubMed ID: 30236627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Hand preferences for bimanual coordination in 29 bonobos (Pan paniscus).
    Chapelain AS; Hogervorst E
    Behav Brain Res; 2009 Jan; 196(1):15-29. PubMed ID: 18692095
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Manipulative repertoire of bonobos (Pan paniscus) in spontaneous feeding situation.
    Gérard C; Bardo A; Guéry JP; Pouydebat E; Simmen B; Narat V
    Am J Primatol; 2022 Jul; 84(7):e23383. PubMed ID: 35417066
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Sex differences in tool use acquisition in bonobos (Pan paniscus).
    Boose KJ; White FJ; Meinelt A
    Am J Primatol; 2013 Sep; 75(9):917-26. PubMed ID: 23606188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Comparing infant and juvenile behavior in bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): a preliminary study.
    De Lathouwers M; Van Elsacker L
    Primates; 2006 Oct; 47(4):287-93. PubMed ID: 16683056
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Segmental morphometrics of bonobos (Pan paniscus): are they really different from chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)?
    Druelle F; Schoonaert K; Aerts P; Nauwelaerts S; Stevens JMG; D'Août K
    J Anat; 2018 Dec; 233(6):843-853. PubMed ID: 30294787
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Mammals consumed by bonobos (Pan paniscus): new data from the Iyondji forest, Tshuapa, Democratic Republic of the Congo.
    Sakamaki T; Maloueki U; Bakaa B; Bongoli L; Kasalevo P; Terada S; Furuichi T
    Primates; 2016 Jul; 57(3):295-301. PubMed ID: 26968409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 16.