These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

150 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27598059)

  • 21. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) let lesser rewards pass them by to get better rewards.
    Bramlett JL; Perdue BM; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Sep; 15(5):963-9. PubMed ID: 22689285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Language-trained chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) delay gratification by choosing token exchange over immediate reward consumption.
    Beran MJ; Evans TA
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Sep; 74(9):864-70. PubMed ID: 22674686
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Testing brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus) on the reverse-reward contingency task without a modified procedure.
    Genty E; Chung PC; Roeder JJ
    Behav Processes; 2011 Jan; 86(1):133-7. PubMed ID: 21044665
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Maintenance of self-imposed delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus).
    Beran MJ
    J Gen Psychol; 2002 Jan; 129(1):49-66. PubMed ID: 12038494
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Visual preference by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) for photos of primates measured by a free choice-order task: implication for influence of social experience.
    Tanaka M
    Primates; 2003 Apr; 44(2):157-65. PubMed ID: 12687480
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Any reward will do: Effects of a reverse-reward contingency on size preference with pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).
    Fernand JK; Amanieh H; Cox DJ; Dorey NR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):472-478. PubMed ID: 30120727
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Why do distractions sometimes aid self-control? Pigeons (Columba livia) highlight possible mechanisms underlying the distraction effect.
    Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2023 Aug; 137(3):145-147. PubMed ID: 37639231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. How the great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla) perform on the reversed reward contingency task II: transfer to new quantities, long-term retention, and the impact of quantity ratios.
    Uher J; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):204-12. PubMed ID: 18489236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Waiting by mistake: symbolic representation of rewards modulates intertemporal choice in capuchin monkeys, preschool children and adult humans.
    Addessi E; Bellagamba F; Delfino A; De Petrillo F; Focaroli V; Macchitella L; Maggiorelli V; Pace B; Pecora G; Rossi S; Sbaffi A; Tasselli MI; Paglieri F
    Cognition; 2014 Mar; 130(3):428-41. PubMed ID: 24387915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Numerical judgments by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in a token economy.
    Beran MJ; Evans TA; Hoyle D
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2011 Apr; 37(2):165-74. PubMed ID: 21319916
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Delay of gratification by orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in the accumulation task.
    Parrish AE; Perdue BM; Stromberg EE; Bania AE; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 May; 128(2):209-14. PubMed ID: 24611642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Responses to a simple barter task in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes.
    Brosnan SF; de Waal FB
    Primates; 2005 Jul; 46(3):173-82. PubMed ID: 15824938
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Dealing with interference: Chimpanzees respond to conflicting cues in a food-choice memory task.
    Parrish AE; Otalora-Garcia A; Beran MJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2017 Oct; 43(4):366-376. PubMed ID: 28981310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Responses to quantity: perceptual versus cognitive mechanisms in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Boysen ST; Berntson GG
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1995 Jan; 21(1):82-6. PubMed ID: 7844508
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. State-transition-free reinforcement learning in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).
    Sato Y; Sakai Y; Hirata S
    Learn Behav; 2023 Dec; 51(4):413-427. PubMed ID: 37369920
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Can lemurs (Eulemur fulvus and E. macaco) use abstract representations of quantities to master the reverse-reward contingency task?
    Genty E; Roeder JJ
    Primates; 2011 Jul; 52(3):253-60. PubMed ID: 21394424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Perception of food amounts by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the role of magnitude, contiguity, and wholeness.
    Beran MJ; Evans TA; Ratliff CL
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Oct; 35(4):516-24. PubMed ID: 19839704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. The relationship between problem solving and inhibitory control: cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus) performance on a reversed contingency task.
    Kralik JD; Hauser MD; Zimlicki R
    J Comp Psychol; 2002 Mar; 116(1):39-50. PubMed ID: 11926684
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Chimpanzees' understanding of social leverage.
    Sánchez-Amaro A; Duguid S; Call J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(12):e0207868. PubMed ID: 30540763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Prospective memory in children and chimpanzees.
    Perdue BM; Evans TA; Williamson RA; Gonsiorowski A; Beran MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):287-95. PubMed ID: 23884791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.