BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

220 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27667899)

  • 1. Grammatical Constraints on Language Switching: Language Control is not Just Executive Control.
    Gollan TH; Goldrick M
    J Mem Lang; 2016 Oct; 90():177-199. PubMed ID: 27667899
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. A switch is not a switch: Syntactically-driven bilingual language control.
    Gollan TH; Goldrick M
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Jan; 44(1):143-156. PubMed ID: 28782969
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Intact reversed language-dominance but exaggerated cognate effects in reading aloud of language switches in bilingual Alzheimer's disease.
    Gollan TH; Li C; Stasenko A; Salmon DP
    Neuropsychology; 2020 Jan; 34(1):88-106. PubMed ID: 31545627
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Multiple levels of bilingual language control: evidence from language intrusions in reading aloud.
    Gollan TH; Schotter ER; Gomez J; Murillo M; Rayner K
    Psychol Sci; 2014 Feb; 25(2):585-95. PubMed ID: 24367061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Cognates interfere with language selection but enhance monitoring in connected speech.
    Li C; Gollan TH
    Mem Cognit; 2018 Aug; 46(6):923-939. PubMed ID: 29679293
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Bilingual language intrusions and other speech errors in Alzheimer's disease.
    Gollan TH; Stasenko A; Li C; Salmon DP
    Brain Cogn; 2017 Nov; 118():27-44. PubMed ID: 28753438
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Using what's there: Bilinguals adaptively rely on orthographic and color cues to achieve language control.
    Fadlon J; Li C; Prior A; Gollan TH
    Cognition; 2019 Oct; 191():103990. PubMed ID: 31376660
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Minimal Overlap in Language Control Across Production And Comprehension: Evidence from Read-Aloud Versus Eye-Tracking Tasks.
    Ahn D; Abbott MJ; Rayner K; Ferreira VS; Gollan TH
    J Neurolinguistics; 2020 May; 54():. PubMed ID: 32189830
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Inhibitory control of the dominant language: Reversed language dominance is the tip of the iceberg.
    Goldrick M; Gollan TH
    J Mem Lang; 2023 Jun; 130():. PubMed ID: 36873561
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection.
    Gollan TH; Sandoval T; Salmon DP
    Psychol Sci; 2011 Sep; 22(9):1155-64. PubMed ID: 21775653
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. What reading aloud reveals about speaking: Regressive saccades implicate a failure to monitor, not inattention, in the prevalence of intrusion errors on function words.
    Schotter ER; Li C; Gollan TH
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2019 Aug; 72(8):2032-2045. PubMed ID: 30509156
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Different language control mechanisms in comprehension and production: Evidence from paragraph reading.
    Li C; Midgley KJ; Ferreira VS; Holcomb PJ; Gollan TH
    Brain Lang; 2024 Jan; 248():105367. PubMed ID: 38113600
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. When Language Switching has No Apparent Cost: Lexical Access in Sentence Context.
    Gullifer JW; Kroll JF; Dussias PE
    Front Psychol; 2013; 4():278. PubMed ID: 23750141
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Should I stay or should I switch? A cost-benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals.
    Gollan TH; Ferreira VS
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2009 May; 35(3):640-65. PubMed ID: 19379041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. What Cognates Reveal about Default Language Selection in Bilingual Sentence Production.
    Li C; Gollan TH
    J Mem Lang; 2021 Jun; 118():. PubMed ID: 33456132
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter.
    Yow WQ; Li X
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():164. PubMed ID: 25767451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Cognate Costs in Bilingual Speech Production: Evidence from Language Switching.
    Broersma M; Carter D; Acheson DJ
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():1461. PubMed ID: 27733838
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Grammatical Encoding in Bilingual Language Production: A Focus on Code-switching.
    Purmohammad M
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():1797. PubMed ID: 26635695
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Natural vs forced language switching: Free selection and consistent language use eliminate significant performance costs and cognitive demands in the brain.
    Zhu JD; Blanco-Elorrieta E; Sun Y; Szakay A; Sowman PF
    Neuroimage; 2022 Feb; 247():118797. PubMed ID: 34896585
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: evidence from Spanish-English and Mandarin-English bilinguals.
    Prior A; Gollan TH
    J Int Neuropsychol Soc; 2011 Jul; 17(4):682-91. PubMed ID: 22882810
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.