These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

99 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27709968)

  • 21. Eurasian jays do not copy the choices of conspecifics, but they do show evidence of stimulus enhancement.
    Miller R; Logan CJ; Lister K; Clayton NS
    PeerJ; 2016; 4():e2746. PubMed ID: 27920957
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Cache protection strategies by western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica): hiding food in the shade.
    Dally JM; Emery NJ; Clayton NS
    Proc Biol Sci; 2004 Dec; 271 Suppl 6(Suppl 6):S387-90. PubMed ID: 15801583
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Eavesdropping squirrels reduce their future value of food under the perceived presence of cache robbers.
    Schmidt KA; Ostfeld RS
    Am Nat; 2008 Mar; 171(3):386-93. PubMed ID: 18220481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Mirror-mediated string-pulling task in Eurasian jays (Garrulus glandarius).
    Baciadonna L; Cornero FM; Clayton NS; Emery NJ
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Jun; 25(3):691-700. PubMed ID: 34913110
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Who's watching influences caching effort in wild Steller's jays (Cyanocitta stelleri).
    Kalinowski RS; Gabriel PO; Black JM
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):95-8. PubMed ID: 25001401
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Hint-seeking behaviour of western scrub-jays in a metacognition task.
    Watanabe A; Clayton NS
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Jan; 19(1):53-64. PubMed ID: 26267805
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. To eat or not to eat. The effects of expectancy on reactivity to food cues.
    Hardman CA; Scott J; Field M; Jones A
    Appetite; 2014 May; 76():153-60. PubMed ID: 24530655
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) are sensitive to distance, but not lighting when caching in the presence of a conspecific.
    Tornick JK; Rushia SN; Gibson BM
    Behav Processes; 2016 Feb; 123():125-33. PubMed ID: 26581321
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Are Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) able to discriminate knowledge states of human experimenters during an object-choice task?
    Clary D; Kelly DM
    Evol Psychol; 2013 Jul; 11(3):628-46. PubMed ID: 23864297
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Waiting for a better possibility: delay of gratification in corvids and its relationship to other cognitive capacities.
    Schnell AK; Boeckle M; Clayton NS
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2022 Dec; 377(1866):20210348. PubMed ID: 36314150
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Tool-use and instrumental learning in the Eurasian jay (Garrulus glandarius).
    Cheke LG; Bird CD; Clayton NS
    Anim Cogn; 2011 May; 14(3):441-55. PubMed ID: 21249510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Social inhibitory control in five lemur species.
    Reddy RB; MacLean EL; Sandel AA; Hare B
    Primates; 2015 Jul; 56(3):241-52. PubMed ID: 25822664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Mental time travel: animals anticipate the future.
    Roberts WA
    Curr Biol; 2007 Jun; 17(11):R418-20. PubMed ID: 17550769
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The domain specificity of intertemporal choice in pinyon jays.
    Stevens JR; Kennedy BA; Morales D; Burks M
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2016 Jun; 23(3):915-21. PubMed ID: 26620957
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Eurasian jays predict the food preferences of their mates.
    Kamil AC
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2013 Mar; 110(10):3719-20. PubMed ID: 23426632
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Azure-winged magpies' decisions to share food are contingent on the presence or absence of food for the recipient.
    Massen JJM; Haley SM; Bugnyar T
    Sci Rep; 2020 Sep; 10(1):16147. PubMed ID: 32999416
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Effects of experience and social context on prospective caching strategies by scrub jays.
    Emery NJ; Clayton NS
    Nature; 2001 Nov; 414(6862):443-6. PubMed ID: 11719804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. The Role of Experimenter Belief in Social Priming.
    Gilder TSE; Heerey EA
    Psychol Sci; 2018 Mar; 29(3):403-417. PubMed ID: 29377787
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Sex, estradiol, and spatial memory in a food-caching corvid.
    Rensel MA; Ellis JM; Harvey B; Schlinger BA
    Horm Behav; 2015 Sep; 75():45-54. PubMed ID: 26232613
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Early reasoning about desires: evidence from 14- and 18-month-olds.
    Repacholi BM; Gopnik A
    Dev Psychol; 1997 Jan; 33(1):12-21. PubMed ID: 9050386
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 5.