These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

138 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27757085)

  • 21. Testing the social dog hypothesis: are dogs also more skilled than chimpanzees in non-communicative social tasks?
    Wobber V; Hare B
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):423-8. PubMed ID: 19376207
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Similarity between an unfamiliar human and the owner affects dogs' preference for human partner when responding to an unsolvable problem.
    Kiss O; Kovács K; Szánthó F; Topál J
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):430-441. PubMed ID: 30022446
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Social learning from conspecifics and humans in dog puppies.
    Fugazza C; Moesta A; Pogány Á; Miklósi Á
    Sci Rep; 2018 Jul; 8(1):9257. PubMed ID: 29977034
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Dogs and their human companions: the effect of familiarity on dog-human interactions.
    Kerepesi A; Dóka A; Miklósi Á
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():27-36. PubMed ID: 24548652
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Behavioral and Neurophysiological Correlates of Dogs' Individual Sensitivities to Being Observed by Their Owners While Performing a Repetitive Fetching Task.
    Kiss O; Kis A; Scheiling K; Topál J
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():1461. PubMed ID: 32760316
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Individual differences in social and non-social behaviors in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) during the acquisition, extinction and reacquisition of a problem solving task.
    Shimabukuro C; Putrino N; Helbling J; Tognetti S; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2015 Apr; 113():179-86. PubMed ID: 25682735
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Do Dogs Prefer Helpers in an Infant-Based Social Evaluation Task?
    McAuliffe K; Bogese M; Chang LW; Andrews CE; Mayer T; Faranda A; Hamlin JK; Santos LR
    Front Psychol; 2019; 10():591. PubMed ID: 30984062
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Do Dogs Provide Information Helpfully?
    Piotti P; Kaminski J
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(8):e0159797. PubMed ID: 27508932
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. The Effect of Domestication and Experience on the Social Interaction of Dogs and Wolves With a Human Companion.
    Lazzaroni M; Range F; Backes J; Portele K; Scheck K; Marshall-Pescini S
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():785. PubMed ID: 32395114
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Behavioural coordination of dogs in a cooperative problem-solving task with a conspecific and a human partner.
    Ostojić L; Clayton NS
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Mar; 17(2):445-59. PubMed ID: 23995845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Neurobiological underpinnings of dogs' human-like social competence: How interactions between stress response systems and oxytocin mediate dogs' social skills.
    Buttner AP
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2016 Dec; 71():198-214. PubMed ID: 27593441
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. A task-experienced partner does not help dogs be as successful as wolves in a cooperative string-pulling task.
    Marshall-Pescini S; Basin C; Range F
    Sci Rep; 2018 Oct; 8(1):16049. PubMed ID: 30375414
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Are dogs able to communicate with their owners about a desirable food in a referential and intentional way?
    Savalli C; Ades C; Gaunet F
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(9):e108003. PubMed ID: 25232956
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. The role of domestication and experience in 'looking back' towards humans in an unsolvable task.
    Marshall-Pescini S; Rao A; Virányi Z; Range F
    Sci Rep; 2017 Apr; 7():46636. PubMed ID: 28422169
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Agility and search and rescue training differently affects pet dogs' behaviour in socio-cognitive tasks.
    Marshall-Pescini S; Passalacqua C; Barnard S; Valsecchi P; Prato-Previde E
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):416-22. PubMed ID: 19520242
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Life experience rather than domestication accounts for dogs' increased oxytocin release during social contact with humans.
    Wirobski G; Range F; Schaebs FS; Palme R; Deschner T; Marshall-Pescini S
    Sci Rep; 2021 Jul; 11(1):14423. PubMed ID: 34257399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Social learning from humans or conspecifics: differences and similarities between wolves and dogs.
    Range F; Virányi Z
    Front Psychol; 2013; 4():868. PubMed ID: 24363648
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Adapting to the human world: dogs' responsiveness to our social cues.
    Reid PJ
    Behav Processes; 2009 Mar; 80(3):325-33. PubMed ID: 19056474
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Intranasal oxytocin and a polymorphism in the oxytocin receptor gene are associated with human-directed social behavior in golden retriever dogs.
    Persson ME; Trottier AJ; Bélteky J; Roth LSV; Jensen P
    Horm Behav; 2017 Sep; 95():85-93. PubMed ID: 28765081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Investigating Empathy-Like Responding to Conspecifics' Distress in Pet Dogs.
    Quervel-Chaumette M; Faerber V; Faragó T; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(4):e0152920. PubMed ID: 27124485
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.