These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

345 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27875540)

  • 1. Improving Preschoolers' Arithmetic through Number Magnitude Training: The Impact of Non-Symbolic and Symbolic Training.
    Honoré N; Noël MP
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(11):e0166685. PubMed ID: 27875540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Preschoolers prior formal mathematics education engage numerical magnitude representation rather than counting principles in symbolic ±1 arithmetic: Evidence from the operational momentum effect.
    Haman M; Lipowska K
    Dev Sci; 2023 May; 26(3):e13322. PubMed ID: 36069221
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Effects of non-symbolic arithmetic training on symbolic arithmetic and the approximate number system.
    Au J; Jaeggi SM; Buschkuehl M
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2018 Apr; 185():1-12. PubMed ID: 29407240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Foundations of children's numerical and mathematical skills: the roles of symbolic and nonsymbolic representations of numerical magnitude.
    Lyons IM; Ansari D
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2015; 48():93-116. PubMed ID: 25735942
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Differences in arithmetic performance between Chinese and German adults are accompanied by differences in processing of non-symbolic numerical magnitude.
    Lonnemann J; Li S; Zhao P; Li P; Linkersdörfer J; Lindberg S; Hasselhorn M; Yan S
    PLoS One; 2017; 12(4):e0174991. PubMed ID: 28384191
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Working memory and number line representations in single-digit addition: Approximate versus exact, nonsymbolic versus symbolic.
    Xenidou-Dervou I; van der Schoot M; van Lieshout EC
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2015; 68(6):1148-67. PubMed ID: 25337863
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Improving arithmetic performance with number sense training: an investigation of underlying mechanism.
    Park J; Brannon EM
    Cognition; 2014 Oct; 133(1):188-200. PubMed ID: 25044247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. What basic number processing measures in kindergarten explain unique variability in first-grade arithmetic proficiency?
    Bartelet D; Vaessen A; Blomert L; Ansari D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Jan; 117():12-28. PubMed ID: 24128690
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Disentangling the Mechanisms of Symbolic Number Processing in Adults' Mathematics and Arithmetic Achievement.
    Orrantia J; Muñez D; Matilla L; Sanchez R; San Romualdo S; Verschaffel L
    Cogn Sci; 2019 Jan; 43(1):. PubMed ID: 30648799
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The relationship between numerical magnitude processing and math anxiety, and their joint effect on adult math performance, varied by indicators of numerical tasks.
    Szczygieł M; Sarı MH
    Cogn Process; 2024 Aug; 25(3):421-442. PubMed ID: 38644404
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Working memory in nonsymbolic approximate arithmetic processing: a dual-task study with preschoolers.
    Xenidou-Dervou I; van Lieshout EC; van der Schoot M
    Cogn Sci; 2014; 38(1):101-27. PubMed ID: 23855416
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Failure to replicate the benefit of approximate arithmetic training for symbolic arithmetic fluency in adults.
    Szkudlarek E; Park J; Brannon EM
    Cognition; 2021 Feb; 207():104521. PubMed ID: 33280814
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Changing the precision of preschoolers' approximate number system representations changes their symbolic math performance.
    Wang JJ; Odic D; Halberda J; Feigenson L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Jul; 147():82-99. PubMed ID: 27061668
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Differential processing of symbolic numerical magnitude and order in first-grade children.
    Vogel SE; Remark A; Ansari D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Jan; 129():26-39. PubMed ID: 25240153
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Narrowing the Early Mathematics Gap: A Play-Based Intervention to Promote Low-Income Preschoolers' Number Skills.
    Scalise NR; Daubert EN; Ramani GB
    J Numer Cogn; 2017; 3(3):559-581. PubMed ID: 34553016
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The Acuity and Manipulability of the ANS Have Separable Influences on Preschoolers' Symbolic Math Achievement.
    Starr A; Tomlinson RC; Brannon EM
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():2554. PubMed ID: 30618975
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Children's expectations about training the approximate number system.
    Dillon MR; Pires AC; Hyde DC; Spelke ES
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2015 Nov; 33(4):411-8. PubMed ID: 26399713
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Testing the Efficacy of Training Basic Numerical Cognition and Transfer Effects to Improvement in Children's Math Ability.
    Kim N; Jang S; Cho S
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():1775. PubMed ID: 30333768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Improving number abilities in low achieving preschoolers: Symbolic versus non-symbolic training programs.
    Van Herwegen J; Costa HM; Nicholson B; Donlan C
    Res Dev Disabil; 2018 Jun; 77():1-11. PubMed ID: 29614401
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. No evidence of learning in non-symbolic numerical tasks--A comment on Park and Brannon (2014).
    Lindskog M; Winman A
    Cognition; 2016 May; 150():243-7. PubMed ID: 26972468
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 18.