These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

298 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27882595)

  • 1. Children's Representation and Imitation of Events: How Goal Organization Influences 3-Year-Old Children's Memory for Action Sequences.
    Loucks J; Mutschler C; Meltzoff AN
    Cogn Sci; 2017 Sep; 41(7):1904-1933. PubMed ID: 27882595
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Goals influence memory and imitation for dynamic human action in 36-month-old children.
    Loucks J; Meltzoff AN
    Scand J Psychol; 2013 Feb; 54(1):41-50. PubMed ID: 23121600
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Memory for temporal order in action is slow developing, sensitive to deviant input, and supported by foundational cognitive processes.
    Loucks J; Price HL
    Dev Psychol; 2019 Feb; 55(2):263-273. PubMed ID: 30407022
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The cognitive structure of goal emulation during the preschool years.
    Subiaul F; Patterson EM; Barr R
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2016 Mar; 34(1):132-49. PubMed ID: 26338371
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Rational imitation declines within the second year of life: Changes in the function of imitation.
    Gellén K; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Sep; 185():148-163. PubMed ID: 31153127
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Age-related changes in learning across early childhood: a new imitation task.
    Dickerson K; Gerhardstein P; Zack E; Barr R
    Dev Psychobiol; 2013 Nov; 55(7):719-32. PubMed ID: 22786801
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The planning and execution of natural sequential actions in the preschool years.
    Freier L; Cooper RP; Mareschal D
    Cognition; 2015 Nov; 144():58-66. PubMed ID: 26232585
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior.
    Over H; Carpenter M
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 May; 126(2):182-92. PubMed ID: 21767011
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The Rationality of (Over)imitation.
    Keupp S; Behne T; Rakoczy H
    Perspect Psychol Sci; 2018 Nov; 13(6):678-687. PubMed ID: 30301424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Children's use of linguistic information when learning in a bilingual context.
    Atagi N; Goldenberg ER; Sandhofer CM
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Apr; 144():199-208. PubMed ID: 26687441
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Measuring action understanding: Relations between goal prediction and imitation.
    Gampe A; Prinz W; Daum MM
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2016 Mar; 34(1):53-65. PubMed ID: 26708448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Social learning promotes understanding of the physical world: Preschool children's imitation of weight sorting.
    Wang Z; Meltzoff AN; Williamson RA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Aug; 136():82-91. PubMed ID: 25866145
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Imitation of a bimanual task in preschool- and school-age children: a hierarchical construction.
    Labiadh L; Ramanantsoa MM; Golomer E
    J Electromyogr Kinesiol; 2012 Aug; 22(4):513-9. PubMed ID: 22391341
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Towards a developmental cognitive science. The implications of cross-modal matching and imitation for the development of representation and memory in infancy.
    Meltzoff AN
    Ann N Y Acad Sci; 1990; 608():1-31; discussion 31-7. PubMed ID: 2075949
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds' imitation from live and televised models.
    Nielsen M; Simcock G; Jenkins L
    Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):722-31. PubMed ID: 18801128
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Beyond irrelevant actions: understanding the role of intentionality in children's imitation of relevant actions.
    Gardiner AK
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Mar; 119():54-72. PubMed ID: 24309246
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Seeing triggers acting, hearing does not trigger saying: evidence from children's weak inhibition.
    Simpson A; Cooper NR; Gillmeister H; Riggs KJ
    Cognition; 2013 Aug; 128(2):103-12. PubMed ID: 23669048
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. How self-generated labelling shapes transfer of learning during early childhood: The role of individual differences.
    Moser A; Olsen S; Rusnak SN; Barr R; Gerhardstein P
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2019 Mar; 37(1):68-83. PubMed ID: 29981173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Over-imitation is not automatic: context sensitivity in children's overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions.
    Keupp S; Behne T; Zachow J; Kasbohm A; Rakoczy H
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Feb; 130():163-75. PubMed ID: 25462039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. From imitation to implementation: How two- and three-year-old children learn to enforce social norms.
    Hardecker S; Tomasello M
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2017 Jun; 35(2):237-248. PubMed ID: 27653172
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 15.