These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

149 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27893268)

  • 1. Domain-general mechanisms for speech segmentation: The role of duration information in language learning.
    Frost RL; Monaghan P; Tatsumi T
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2017 Mar; 43(3):466-476. PubMed ID: 27893268
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Perception of rhythmic grouping: testing the iambic/trochaic law.
    Hay JS; Diehl RL
    Percept Psychophys; 2007 Jan; 69(1):113-22. PubMed ID: 17515221
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Speech segmentation is facilitated by visual cues.
    Cunillera T; Camara E; Laine M; Rodriguez-Fornells A
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2010 Feb; 63(2):260-74. PubMed ID: 19526435
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Rhythmic grouping biases constrain infant statistical learning.
    Hay JF; Saffran JR
    Infancy; 2012 Nov; 17(6):610-641. PubMed ID: 23730217
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Distal prosody affects learning of novel words in an artificial language.
    Morrill TH; McAuley JD; Dilley LC; Zdziarska PA; Jones KB; Sanders LD
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2015 Jun; 22(3):815-23. PubMed ID: 25245269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Language specific prosodic preferences during the first half year of life: evidence from German and French infants.
    Höhle B; Bijeljac-Babic R; Herold B; Weissenborn J; Nazzi T
    Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Jun; 32(3):262-74. PubMed ID: 19427039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. An Exploration of Rhythmic Grouping of Speech Sequences by French- and German-Learning Infants.
    Abboub N; Boll-Avetisyan N; Bhatara A; Höhle B; Nazzi T
    Front Hum Neurosci; 2016; 10():292. PubMed ID: 27378887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Native language affects rhythmic grouping of speech.
    Bhatara A; Boll-Avetisyan N; Unger A; Nazzi T; Höhle B
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2013 Nov; 134(5):3828-43. PubMed ID: 24180792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Do humans and nonhuman animals share the grouping principles of the iambic-trochaic law?
    de la Mora DM; Nespor M; Toro JM
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2013 Jan; 75(1):92-100. PubMed ID: 22956287
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The relationship between language production and verbal short-term memory: the role of stress grouping.
    Morgan JL; Edwards S; Wheeldon LR
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2014 Feb; 67(2):220-46. PubMed ID: 23745759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Cross-linguistic differences in the use of durational cues for the segmentation of a novel language.
    Ordin M; Polyanskaya L; Laka I; Nespor M
    Mem Cognit; 2017 Jul; 45(5):863-876. PubMed ID: 28290103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Perceptual tests of rhythmic similarity: I. Mora rhythm.
    Murty L; Otake T; Cutler A
    Lang Speech; 2007; 50(Pt 1):77-99. PubMed ID: 17518104
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. More than a boundary shift: Perceptual adaptation to foreign-accented speech reshapes the internal structure of phonetic categories.
    Xie X; Theodore RM; Myers EB
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2017 Jan; 43(1):206-217. PubMed ID: 27819457
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Language dominance shapes non-linguistic rhythmic grouping in bilinguals.
    Molnar M; Carreiras M; Gervain J
    Cognition; 2016 Jul; 152():150-159. PubMed ID: 27062227
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Perception of final fricative voicing: native and nonnative listeners' use of vowel duration.
    Broersma M
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2010 Mar; 127(3):1636-44. PubMed ID: 20329862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Learning a Phonological Contrast Modulates the Auditory Grouping of Rhythm.
    Henny Yeung H; Bhatara A; Nazzi T
    Cogn Sci; 2018 Jun; ():. PubMed ID: 29923619
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Development of infants' segmentation of words from native speech: a meta-analytic approach.
    Bergmann C; Cristia A
    Dev Sci; 2016 Nov; 19(6):901-917. PubMed ID: 26353859
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Experience with a second language affects the use of fundamental frequency in speech segmentation.
    Tremblay A; Namjoshi J; Spinelli E; Broersma M; Cho T; Kim S; Martínez-García MT; Connell K
    PLoS One; 2017; 12(7):e0181709. PubMed ID: 28738093
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Training the brain to weight speech cues differently: a study of Finnish second-language users of English.
    Ylinen S; Uther M; Latvala A; Vepsäläinen S; Iverson P; Akahane-Yamada R; Näätänen R
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2010 Jun; 22(6):1319-32. PubMed ID: 19445609
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.