These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

148 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 27966800)

  • 1. Individual Differences in Children's Corepresentation of Self and Other in Joint Action.
    Milward SJ; Kita S; Apperly IA
    Child Dev; 2017 May; 88(3):964-978. PubMed ID: 27966800
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Individual differences in the effectiveness of self-distancing for young children's emotion regulation.
    Grenell A; Prager EO; Schaefer C; Kross E; Duckworth AL; Carlson SM
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2019 Mar; 37(1):84-100. PubMed ID: 30009423
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The contributions of mental state understanding and executive functioning to preschool-aged children's lie-telling.
    Leduc K; Williams S; Gomez-Garibello C; Talwar V
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2017 Jun; 35(2):288-302. PubMed ID: 27774612
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Care to Share? Children's Cognitive Skills and Concealing Responses to a Parent.
    Lavoie J; Talwar V
    Top Cogn Sci; 2020 Apr; 12(2):485-503. PubMed ID: 30370621
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Individual differences in executive function and central coherence predict developmental changes in theory of mind in autism.
    Pellicano E
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Mar; 46(2):530-44. PubMed ID: 20210511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Executive function depletion in children and its impact on theory of mind.
    Powell LJ; Carey S
    Cognition; 2017 Jul; 164():150-162. PubMed ID: 28427031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Development of cool and hot theory of mind and cool and hot inhibitory control abilities from 3.5 to 6.5 years of age.
    Draperi M; Aïte A; Cassotti M; Le Stanc L; Houdé O; Borst G
    PLoS One; 2022; 17(1):e0262251. PubMed ID: 35085269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Cumulative risk disparities in children's neurocognitive functioning: a developmental cascade model.
    Wade M; Browne DT; Plamondon A; Daniel E; Jenkins JM
    Dev Sci; 2016 Mar; 19(2):179-94. PubMed ID: 25845409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The "Batman Effect": Improving Perseverance in Young Children.
    White RE; Prager EO; Schaefer C; Kross E; Duckworth AL; Carlson SM
    Child Dev; 2017 Sep; 88(5):1563-1571. PubMed ID: 27982409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Testing the predictive power of cognitive atypicalities in autistic children: evidence from a 3-year follow-up study.
    Pellicano E
    Autism Res; 2013 Aug; 6(4):258-67. PubMed ID: 23495146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Sibling composition, executive function, and children's thinking about mental diversity.
    Kennedy K; Lagattuta KH; Sayfan L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Apr; 132():121-39. PubMed ID: 25687549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Young Children's Memories for Social Actions: Influences of Age, Theory of Mind, and Motor Complexity.
    Gerson SA; Meyer M
    Child Dev; 2021 Jan; 92(1):142-156. PubMed ID: 32706920
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The link between preschoolers' executive function and theory of mind and the role of epistemic states.
    Henning A; Spinath FM; Aschersleben G
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Mar; 108(3):513-31. PubMed ID: 21118749
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind.
    Carlson SM; Moses LJ
    Child Dev; 2001; 72(4):1032-53. PubMed ID: 11480933
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Does interference between self and other perspectives in theory of mind tasks reflect a common underlying process? Evidence from individual differences in theory of mind and inhibitory control.
    Qureshi AW; Monk RL; Samson D; Apperly IA
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2020 Feb; 27(1):178-190. PubMed ID: 31429057
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers' improvement from theory-of-mind training.
    Benson JE; Sabbagh MA; Carlson SM; Zelazo PD
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Sep; 49(9):1615-27. PubMed ID: 23244411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Theory of mind in middle childhood: Longitudinal associations with executive function and social competence.
    Devine RT; White N; Ensor R; Hughes C
    Dev Psychol; 2016 May; 52(5):758-71. PubMed ID: 26914214
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Contribution of Theory of Mind, Executive Functioning, and Pragmatics to Socialization Behaviors of Children with High-Functioning Autism.
    Berenguer C; Miranda A; Colomer C; Baixauli I; Roselló B
    J Autism Dev Disord; 2018 Feb; 48(2):430-441. PubMed ID: 29027643
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Intelligence and Neurophysiological Markers of Error Monitoring Relate to Children's Intellectual Humility.
    Danovitch JH; Fisher M; Schroder H; Hambrick DZ; Moser J
    Child Dev; 2019 May; 90(3):924-939. PubMed ID: 28922467
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The role of executive functions and theory of mind in children's prosocial lie-telling.
    Williams S; Moore K; Crossman AM; Talwar V
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Jan; 141():256-66. PubMed ID: 26361741
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.