These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

136 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28067417)

  • 21. Cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: the role of representation activation and maintenance.
    Chevalier N; Blaye A
    Dev Sci; 2008 May; 11(3):339-53. PubMed ID: 18466368
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Preschoolers selectively infer history when explaining outcomes: evidence from explanations of ownership, liking, and use.
    Nancekivell SE; Friedman O
    Child Dev; 2014; 85(3):1236-1247. PubMed ID: 24116672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. "Because It's Hers": When Preschoolers Use Ownership in Their Explanations.
    Nancekivell SE; Friedman O
    Cogn Sci; 2017 Apr; 41(3):827-843. PubMed ID: 26936795
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Sibling composition, executive function, and children's thinking about mental diversity.
    Kennedy K; Lagattuta KH; Sayfan L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Apr; 132():121-39. PubMed ID: 25687549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. The "EF" in deficiency: Examining the linkages between executive function and the utilization deficiency observed in preschoolers.
    Stone MM; Blumberg FC; Blair C; Cancelli AA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Dec; 152():367-375. PubMed ID: 27566943
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Repeated sequential action by young children: Developmental changes in representational flexibility of task context.
    Yanaoka K; Saito S
    Dev Psychol; 2019 Apr; 55(4):780-792. PubMed ID: 30640501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Executive function and the development of belief-desire psychology.
    Rakoczy H
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jul; 13(4):648-61. PubMed ID: 20590728
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Removing shared information improves 3- and 4-year-olds' performance on a change-of-location explicit false belief task.
    Salter G; Breheny R
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Nov; 187():104665. PubMed ID: 31409457
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Assessing executive function in preschoolers.
    Anderson PJ; Reidy N
    Neuropsychol Rev; 2012 Dec; 22(4):345-60. PubMed ID: 23109046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. The Multidimensional Card Selection Task: A new way to measure concurrent cognitive flexibility in preschoolers.
    Podjarny G; Kamawar D; Andrews K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jul; 159():199-218. PubMed ID: 28315589
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Counterfactual thinking and false belief: the role of executive function.
    Drayton S; Turley-Ames KJ; Guajardo NR
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Mar; 108(3):532-48. PubMed ID: 21092984
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Attention demands influence 10- and 12-month-old infants' perseverative behavior.
    Watanabe H; Forssman L; Green D; Bohlin G; von Hofsten C
    Dev Psychol; 2012 Jan; 48(1):46-55. PubMed ID: 21910526
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Semantic future thinking and executive functions at age 4: The moderating role of frontal brain electrical activity.
    Blankenship TL; Broomell APR; Ann Bell M
    Dev Psychobiol; 2018 Jul; 60(5):608-614. PubMed ID: 29785731
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. What visual information do children and adults consider while switching between tasks? Eye-tracking investigation of cognitive flexibility development.
    Chevalier N; Blaye A; Dufau S; Lucenet J
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Jul; 46(4):955-72. PubMed ID: 20604615
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. The "Batman Effect": Improving Perseverance in Young Children.
    White RE; Prager EO; Schaefer C; Kross E; Duckworth AL; Carlson SM
    Child Dev; 2017 Sep; 88(5):1563-1571. PubMed ID: 27982409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Two types of perseveration in the Dimension Change Card Sort task.
    Hanania R
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):325-36. PubMed ID: 20566206
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Flankers facilitate 3-year-olds' performance in a card-sorting task.
    Jordan PL; Morton JB
    Dev Psychol; 2008 Jan; 44(1):265-74. PubMed ID: 18194025
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Mature counterfactual reasoning in 4- and 5-year-olds.
    Nyhout A; Ganea PA
    Cognition; 2019 Feb; 183():57-66. PubMed ID: 30414500
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Executive functioning of Portuguese preschoolers in the Shape School test: A cross cultural study.
    Rato JR; Ribeiro F; Castro-Caldas A
    Appl Neuropsychol Child; 2018; 7(3):200-207. PubMed ID: 28631966
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Multidimensional Reasoning Can Promote 3-Year-Old Children's Performance on the Dimensional Change Card Sort Task.
    Bardikoff N; Sabbagh MA
    Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 92(5):e924-e939. PubMed ID: 33496007
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.