These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

113 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28167383)

  • 1. Group presence, category labels, and generic statements influence children to treat descriptive group regularities as prescriptive.
    Roberts SO; Ho AK; Gelman SA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jun; 158():19-31. PubMed ID: 28167383
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. So It Is, So It Shall Be: Group Regularities License Children's Prescriptive Judgments.
    Roberts SO; Gelman SA; Ho AK
    Cogn Sci; 2017 Apr; 41 Suppl 3(Suppl 3):576-600. PubMed ID: 27914116
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Children's descriptive-to-prescriptive tendency replicates (and varies) cross-culturally: Evidence from China.
    Roberts SO; Guo C; Ho AK; Gelman SA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Jan; 165():148-160. PubMed ID: 28552389
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Memory for generic and quantified sentences in Spanish-speaking children and adults.
    Gelman SA; Sánchez Tapia I; Leslie SJ
    J Child Lang; 2016 Nov; 43(6):1231-44. PubMed ID: 26333824
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Follow (or don't follow) the crowd: Young children's conformity is influenced by norm domain and age.
    Flynn E; Turner C; Giraldeau LA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Mar; 167():222-233. PubMed ID: 29190511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Should Individuals Think Like Their Group? A Descriptive-to-Prescriptive Tendency Toward Group-Based Beliefs.
    Roberts SO; Ho AK; Gelman SA
    Child Dev; 2021 Mar; 92(2):e201-e220. PubMed ID: 32845017
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The role of group norms in evaluating uncommon and negative behaviors.
    Roberts SO; Ho AK; Gelman SA
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2019 Feb; 148(2):374-387. PubMed ID: 30570328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Do lions have manes? For children, generics are about kinds rather than quantities.
    Brandone AC; Cimpian A; Leslie SJ; Gelman SA
    Child Dev; 2012; 83(2):423-33. PubMed ID: 22235892
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Preschool children use linguistic form class and pragmatic cues to interpret generics.
    Gelman SA; Raman L
    Child Dev; 2003; 74(1):308-25. PubMed ID: 12625452
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Generic language and speaker confidence guide preschoolers' inferences about novel animate kinds.
    Stock HR; Graham SA; Chambers CG
    Dev Psychol; 2009 May; 45(3):884-8. PubMed ID: 19413440
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Is or ought? Reactions to violations help children to distinguish norms and regularities.
    Riggs AE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Jun; 194():104822. PubMed ID: 32126318
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Development of category-based reasoning in 4- to 7-year-old children: the influence of label co-occurrence and kinship knowledge.
    Godwin KE; Matlen BJ; Fisher AV
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 May; 115(1):74-90. PubMed ID: 23374604
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Category label effects on Chinese children's inductive inferences: modulation by perceptual detail and category specificity.
    Long C; Lu X; Zhang L; Li H; Deák GO
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Feb; 111(2):230-45. PubMed ID: 21945344
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Categories convey prescriptive information across domains and development.
    Foster-Hanson E; Roberts SO; Gelman SA; Rhodes M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2021 Dec; 212():105231. PubMed ID: 34358722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Two- and 3-year-olds integrate linguistic and pedagogical cues in guiding inductive generalization and exploration.
    Butler LP; Tomasello M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 May; 145():64-78. PubMed ID: 26826468
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. How two-year-old children interpret proper and common names for unfamiliar objects.
    Gelman SA; Taylor M
    Child Dev; 1984 Aug; 55(4):1535-40. PubMed ID: 6488963
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. When you've seen one, have you seen them all? Children's memory for general and specific learning episodes.
    Riggs AE; Kalish CW; Alibali MW
    Dev Psychol; 2014 Jun; 50(6):1653-1659. PubMed ID: 24611670
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The influence of linguistic form and causal explanations on the development of social essentialism.
    Benitez J; Leshin RA; Rhodes M
    Cognition; 2022 Dec; 229():105246. PubMed ID: 35985103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Jimmy's baby doll and Jenny's truck: young children's reasoning about gender norms.
    Conry-Murray C; Turiel E
    Child Dev; 2012; 83(1):146-58. PubMed ID: 22171548
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Does the Age and Familiarity of the Informant Group Influence the Tendency of 3- and 4-year-old Children to Conform?
    McGuigan N; Stevenson A
    J Genet Psychol; 2016; 177(4):122-30. PubMed ID: 27341477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.