These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

165 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28318292)

  • 1. From play to proficiency: The ontogeny of stone-tool use in coastal-foraging long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis) from a comparative perception-action perspective.
    Tan AWY
    J Comp Psychol; 2017 May; 131(2):89-114. PubMed ID: 28318292
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Ontogeny of manipulative behavior and nut-cracking in young tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): a perception-action perspective.
    de Resende BD; Ottoni EB; Fragaszy DM
    Dev Sci; 2008 Nov; 11(6):828-40. PubMed ID: 19046151
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The physical characteristics and usage patterns of stone axe and pounding hammers used by long-tailed macaques in the Andaman Sea region of Thailand.
    Gumert MD; Kluck M; Malaivijitnond S
    Am J Primatol; 2009 Jul; 71(7):594-608. PubMed ID: 19405083
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Analysis of sea almond (Terminalia catappa) cracking sites used by wild Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea).
    Falótico T; Spagnoletti N; Haslam M; Luncz LV; Malaivijitnond S; Gumert M
    Am J Primatol; 2017 May; 79(5):. PubMed ID: 28056164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. There Is More than One Way to Crack an Oyster: Identifying Variation in Burmese Long-Tailed Macaque (Macaca fascicularis aurea) Stone-Tool Use.
    Tan A; Tan SH; Vyas D; Malaivijitnond S; Gumert MD
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(5):e0124733. PubMed ID: 25970286
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Stone-tool usage by Thai long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Malaivijitnond S; Lekprayoon C; Tandavanittj N; Panha S; Cheewatham C; Hamada Y
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Feb; 69(2):227-33. PubMed ID: 17146796
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Marine prey processed with stone tools by Burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea) in intertidal habitats.
    Gumert MD; Malaivijitnond S
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2012 Nov; 149(3):447-57. PubMed ID: 23042618
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Sex differences in the stone tool-use behavior of a wild population of burmese long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis aurea).
    Gumert MD; Hoong LK; Malaivijitnond S
    Am J Primatol; 2011 Dec; 73(12):1239-49. PubMed ID: 21905061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Long-tailed macaques select mass of stone tools according to food type.
    Gumert MD; Malaivijitnond S
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2013 Nov; 368(1630):20120413. PubMed ID: 24101623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Stone affordances as potential for action expression in object play in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis).
    Cenni C; Pellis SM; Wandia IN; Leca JB
    J Comp Psychol; 2021 Aug; 135(3):430-438. PubMed ID: 34553980
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Tool skill impacts the archaeological evidence across technological primates.
    Luncz LV; Slania NE; Almeida-Warren K; Carvalho S; Falótico T; Malaivijitnond S; Arroyo A; de la Torre I; Proffitt T
    Sci Rep; 2024 Jul; 14(1):16556. PubMed ID: 39019910
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Extractive foraging and tool-aided behaviors in the wild Nicobar long-tailed macaque (Macaca fascicularis umbrosus).
    Pal A; Kumara HN; Mishra PS; Velankar AD; Singh M
    Primates; 2018 Mar; 59(2):173-183. PubMed ID: 29086889
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Influence of COVID-19 on the emergence of stone-tool use behavior in a population of common long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis fascicularis) in Thailand.
    Muhammad R; Kaikaew T; Panjan S; Meesawat S; Thabthimthong W; Payungporn S; Apipattarachaiwong J; Kanthaswamy S; Hamada Y; Luncz LV; Malaivijitnond S
    Am J Primatol; 2024 Feb; 86(2):e23580. PubMed ID: 38012960
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Wild capuchin monkeys spontaneously adjust actions when using hammer stones of different mass to crack nuts of different resistance.
    Liu Q; Fragaszy DM; Visalberghi E
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2016 Sep; 161(1):53-61. PubMed ID: 27135182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Resource depletion through primate stone technology.
    Luncz LV; Tan A; Haslam M; Kulik L; Proffitt T; Malaivijitnond S; Gumert M
    Elife; 2017 Sep; 6():. PubMed ID: 28884681
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Stone tool use by adult wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus). Frequency, efficiency and tool selectivity.
    Spagnoletti N; Visalberghi E; Ottoni E; Izar P; Fragaszy D
    J Hum Evol; 2011 Jul; 61(1):97-107. PubMed ID: 21470663
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Characteristics of hammer stones and anvils used by wild bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) to crack open palm nuts.
    Visalberghi E; Fragaszy D; Ottoni E; Izar P; de Oliveira MG; Andrade FR
    Am J Phys Anthropol; 2007 Mar; 132(3):426-44. PubMed ID: 17177182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Linking primatology and archaeology: The transversality of stone percussive behaviors.
    Harmand S; Arroyo A
    J Hum Evol; 2023 Aug; 181():103398. PubMed ID: 37329870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Use-wear patterns on wild macaque stone tools reveal their behavioural history.
    Haslam M; Gumert MD; Biro D; Carvalho S; Malaivijitnond S
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(8):e72872. PubMed ID: 23977365
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Group-specific archaeological signatures of stone tool use in wild macaques.
    Luncz LV; Gill M; Proffitt T; Svensson MS; Kulik L; Malaivijitnond S
    Elife; 2019 Oct; 8():. PubMed ID: 31635691
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.