These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

147 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28345944)

  • 1. Enhanced integration of multisensory body information by proximity to "habitual action space".
    Dempsey-Jones H; Kritikos A
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2017 Apr; 43(4):770-782. PubMed ID: 28345944
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Handedness modulates proprioceptive drift in the rubber hand illusion.
    Dempsey-Jones H; Kritikos A
    Exp Brain Res; 2019 Feb; 237(2):351-361. PubMed ID: 30411222
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The magnetic touch illusion: A perceptual correlate of visuo-tactile integration in peripersonal space.
    Guterstam A; Zeberg H; Özçiftci VM; Ehrsson HH
    Cognition; 2016 Oct; 155():44-56. PubMed ID: 27348406
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The spatial distance rule in the moving and classical rubber hand illusions.
    Kalckert A; Ehrsson HH
    Conscious Cogn; 2014 Nov; 30():118-32. PubMed ID: 25286241
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Proprioceptive body illusions modulate the visual perception of reaching distance.
    Petroni A; Carbajal MJ; Sigman M
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(6):e0131087. PubMed ID: 26110274
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The development of multisensory body representation and awareness continues to 10 years of age: Evidence from the rubber hand illusion.
    Cowie D; Sterling S; Bremner AJ
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Feb; 142():230-8. PubMed ID: 26601752
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Proprioceptive drift is affected by the intermanual distance rather than the distance from the body's midline in the rubber hand illusion.
    Erro R; Marotta A; Fiorio M
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2020 Nov; 82(8):4084-4095. PubMed ID: 32914338
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The role of distance from the body and distance from the real hand in ownership and disownership during the rubber hand illusion.
    Preston C
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2013 Feb; 142(2):177-83. PubMed ID: 23333877
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Body ownership and agency: task-dependent effects of the virtual hand illusion on proprioceptive drift.
    Shibuya S; Unenaka S; Ohki Y
    Exp Brain Res; 2017 Jan; 235(1):121-134. PubMed ID: 27651139
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Changes in perceived peripersonal space following the rubber hand illusion.
    Smit M; Dijkerman HC; Kurstjens V; de Haan AM; van der Ham IJM; van der Smagt MJ
    Sci Rep; 2023 May; 13(1):7713. PubMed ID: 37173340
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Unconscious integration of multisensory bodily inputs in the peripersonal space shapes bodily self-consciousness.
    Salomon R; Noel JP; Łukowska M; Faivre N; Metzinger T; Serino A; Blanke O
    Cognition; 2017 Sep; 166():174-183. PubMed ID: 28577447
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Parkinson's disease alters multisensory perception: Insights from the Rubber Hand Illusion.
    Ding C; Palmer CJ; Hohwy J; Youssef GJ; Paton B; Tsuchiya N; Stout JC; Thyagarajan D
    Neuropsychologia; 2017 Mar; 97():38-45. PubMed ID: 28153639
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Vestibular contributions to a right-hemisphere network for bodily awareness: combining galvanic vestibular stimulation and the "Rubber Hand Illusion".
    Ferrè ER; Berlot E; Haggard P
    Neuropsychologia; 2015 Mar; 69():140-7. PubMed ID: 25619847
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The effect of limb crossing and limb congruency on multisensory integration in peripersonal space for the upper and lower extremities.
    van Elk M; Forget J; Blanke O
    Conscious Cogn; 2013 Jun; 22(2):545-55. PubMed ID: 23579198
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The robot hand illusion: inducing proprioceptive drift through visuo-motor congruency.
    Romano D; Caffa E; Hernandez-Arieta A; Brugger P; Maravita A
    Neuropsychologia; 2015 Apr; 70():414-20. PubMed ID: 25446964
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Temperament and psychopathological syndromes specific susceptibility for rubber hand illusion.
    Kállai J; Hegedüs G; Feldmann Á; Rózsa S; Darnai G; Herold R; Dorn K; Kincses P; Csathó Á; Szolcsányi T
    Psychiatry Res; 2015 Sep; 229(1-2):410-9. PubMed ID: 26160198
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Multisensory integration across exteroceptive and interoceptive domains modulates self-experience in the rubber-hand illusion.
    Suzuki K; Garfinkel SN; Critchley HD; Seth AK
    Neuropsychologia; 2013 Nov; 51(13):2909-17. PubMed ID: 23993906
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Direction-dependent integration of vision and proprioception in reaching under the influence of the mirror illusion.
    Snijders HJ; Holmes NP; Spence C
    Neuropsychologia; 2007 Feb; 45(3):496-505. PubMed ID: 16499935
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Spatial limits on the nonvisual self-touch illusion and the visual rubber hand illusion: subjective experience of the illusion and proprioceptive drift.
    Aimola Davies AM; White RC; Davies M
    Conscious Cogn; 2013 Jun; 22(2):613-36. PubMed ID: 23644413
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Lost in space: multisensory conflict yields adaptation in spatial representations across frames of reference.
    Lohmann J; Butz MV
    Cogn Process; 2017 Aug; 18(3):211-228. PubMed ID: 28349249
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.