These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
140 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28383940)
1. Causal superlearning arising from interactions among cues. Urushihara K; Miller RR J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2017 Apr; 43(2):183-196. PubMed ID: 28383940 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Stimulus competition between a discrete cue and a training context: Cue competition does not result from the division of a limited resource. Urushihara K; Miller RR J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Apr; 35(2):197-211. PubMed ID: 19364229 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Blocking is not 'pure' cue competition: Renewal-like effects in forward and backward blocking indicate contributions by associative cue interference. Miguez G; Miller RR J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2022 Apr; 48(2):145-159. PubMed ID: 35225640 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Associative foundation of causal learning in rats. Polack CW; McConnell BL; Miller RR Learn Behav; 2013 Mar; 41(1):25-41. PubMed ID: 22562460 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Stimulus preexposure speeds or slows subsequent acquisition of associative learning depending on learning test procedures and response measure. Holland PC Learn Behav; 2018 Jun; 46(2):134-156. PubMed ID: 29052113 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Retrospective revaluation: The phenomenon and its theoretical implications. Miller RR; Witnauer JE Behav Processes; 2016 Feb; 123():15-25. PubMed ID: 26342855 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Conditioned suppression is an inverted-U function of footshock intensity. Witnauer JE; Miller RR Learn Behav; 2013 Mar; 41(1):94-106. PubMed ID: 22926983 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Associative status of the training context determines the effectiveness of compound extinction. Witnauer JE; Miller RR J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2012 Jan; 38(1):52-65. PubMed ID: 22229586 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. The comparator hypothesis of conditioned response generation: manifest conditioned excitation and inhibition as a function of relative excitatory strengths of CS and conditioning context at the time of testing. Kasprow WJ; Schachtman TR; Miller RR J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1987 Oct; 13(4):395-406. PubMed ID: 3668477 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Experiencing extinction with a non-target cue facilitates reversal of a target conditioned inhibitor in human predictive learning. González G; Alcalá JA; Callejas-Aguilera JE; Rosas JM Behav Processes; 2019 Sep; 166():103898. PubMed ID: 31265879 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Some determinants of second-order conditioning. Witnauer JE; Miller RR Learn Behav; 2011 Mar; 39(1):12-26. PubMed ID: 21264564 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Second-order conditioning and conditioned inhibition: influences of speed versus accuracy on human causal learning. Lee JC; Livesey EJ PLoS One; 2012; 7(11):e49899. PubMed ID: 23209613 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Responding to a conditioned stimulus depends on the current associative status of other cues present during training of that specific stimulus. Miller RR; Barnet RC; Grahame NJ J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1992 Jul; 18(3):251-64. PubMed ID: 1619393 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. The Rescorla-Wagner model: losses in associative strength in compound conditioned stimuli. Kremer EF J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1978 Jan; 4(1):22-36. PubMed ID: 621465 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Recovery from blocking between outcomes. Wheeler DS; Miller RR J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Oct; 31(4):467-76. PubMed ID: 16248732 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]