These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

172 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28460272)

  • 21. Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources.
    Teasdale JD; Dritschel BH; Taylor MJ; Proctor L; Lloyd CA; Nimmo-Smith I; Baddeley AD
    Mem Cognit; 1995 Sep; 23(5):551-9. PubMed ID: 7476241
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Age-related changes in frequency of mind-wandering and task-related interferences during memory encoding and their impact on retrieval.
    Maillet D; Rajah MN
    Memory; 2013; 21(7):818-31. PubMed ID: 23360550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Back to the future: autobiographical planning and the functionality of mind-wandering.
    Baird B; Smallwood J; Schooler JW
    Conscious Cogn; 2011 Dec; 20(4):1604-11. PubMed ID: 21917482
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Dealing with prospective memory demands while performing an ongoing task: Shared processing, increased on-task focus, or both?
    Rummel J; Smeekens BA; Kane MJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2017 Jul; 43(7):1047-1062. PubMed ID: 27936845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Prevalence and determinants of direct and generative modes of production of episodic future thoughts in the word cueing paradigm.
    Jeunehomme O; D'Argembeau A
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2016; 69(2):254-72. PubMed ID: 25436465
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Comparing the phenomenological qualities of stimulus-independent thought, stimulus-dependent thought and dreams using experience sampling.
    Gross ME; Smith AP; Graveline YM; Beaty RE; Schooler JW; Seli P
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2021 Feb; 376(1817):20190694. PubMed ID: 33308068
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Why are we not flooded by involuntary thoughts about the past and future? Testing the cognitive inhibition dependency hypothesis.
    Barzykowski K; Radel R; Niedźwieńska A; Kvavilashvili L
    Psychol Res; 2019 Jun; 83(4):666-683. PubMed ID: 30483873
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Shaped by our thoughts--a new task to assess spontaneous cognition and its associated neural correlates in the default network.
    O'Callaghan C; Shine JM; Lewis SJ; Andrews-Hanna JR; Irish M
    Brain Cogn; 2015 Feb; 93():1-10. PubMed ID: 25463243
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Task-unrelated thought depends on the phonological short-term memory system more than the visual short-term memory system.
    Sekiguchi T
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2018 Oct; 190():228-238. PubMed ID: 30149237
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Controlling the stream of thought: working memory capacity predicts adjustment of mind-wandering to situational demands.
    Rummel J; Boywitt CD
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2014 Oct; 21(5):1309-15. PubMed ID: 24448763
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Motivation, intentionality, and mind wandering: Implications for assessments of task-unrelated thought.
    Seli P; Cheyne JA; Xu M; Purdon C; Smilek D
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2015 Sep; 41(5):1417-1425. PubMed ID: 25730306
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. The role of task difficulty in theoretical accounts of mind wandering.
    Seli P; Konishi M; Risko EF; Smilek D
    Conscious Cogn; 2018 Oct; 65():255-262. PubMed ID: 30219746
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. What did you have in mind? Examining the content of intentional and unintentional types of mind wandering.
    Seli P; Ralph BCW; Konishi M; Smilek D; Schacter DL
    Conscious Cogn; 2017 May; 51():149-156. PubMed ID: 28371688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Concern-induced negative affect is associated with the occurrence and content of mind-wandering.
    Stawarczyk D; Majerus S; D'Argembeau A
    Conscious Cogn; 2013 Jun; 22(2):442-8. PubMed ID: 23466878
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Studying in the region of proximal learning reduces mind wandering.
    Xu J; Metcalfe J
    Mem Cognit; 2016 Jul; 44(5):681-95. PubMed ID: 26849889
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. The wandering self: Tracking distracting self-generated thought in a cognitively demanding context.
    Huijser S; van Vugt MK; Taatgen NA
    Conscious Cogn; 2018 Feb; 58():170-185. PubMed ID: 29275845
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Does episodic future thinking improve prospective remembering?
    Neroni MA; Gamboz N; Brandimonte MA
    Conscious Cogn; 2014 Jan; 23():53-62. PubMed ID: 24368165
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Characterizing the subjective experience of episodic past, future, and counterfactual thinking in healthy younger and older adults.
    De Brigard F; Giovanello KS; Stewart GW; Lockrow AW; O'Brien MM; Spreng RN
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2016 Dec; 69(12):2358-2375. PubMed ID: 27028484
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. A wandering mind is a forgetful mind: A systematic review on the influence of mind wandering on episodic memory encoding.
    Blondé P; Girardeau JC; Sperduti M; Piolino P
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2022 Jan; 132():774-792. PubMed ID: 34906400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Inducing spontaneous future thoughts in younger and older adults by priming future-oriented personal goals.
    Jordão M; Pinho MS; St Jacques PL
    Psychol Res; 2019 Jun; 83(4):710-726. PubMed ID: 30671615
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.