These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

196 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28600922)

  • 1. The object of my desire: Five-year-olds rapidly reason about a speaker's desire during referential communication.
    San Juan V; Chambers CG; Berman J; Humphry C; Graham SA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Oct; 162():101-119. PubMed ID: 28600922
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. When You're Happy and I Know It: Four-Year-Olds' Emotional Perspective Taking During Online Language Comprehension.
    Khu M; Chambers C; Graham SA
    Child Dev; 2018 Nov; 89(6):2264-2281. PubMed ID: 28581688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Preschoolers' sensitivity to speaker action constraints to infer referential intent.
    Collins SJ; Graham SA; Chambers CG
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Aug; 112(4):389-402. PubMed ID: 22651891
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Contextual influences on children's use of vocal affect cues during referential interpretation.
    Berman JM; Graham SA; Chambers CG
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2013; 66(4):705-26. PubMed ID: 22950850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Preschoolers' appreciation of speaker vocal affect as a cue to referential intent.
    Berman JM; Chambers CG; Graham SA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Oct; 107(2):87-99. PubMed ID: 20553796
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Accent, Language, and Race: 4-6-Year-Old Children's Inferences Differ by Speaker Cue.
    Weatherhead D; Friedman O; White KS
    Child Dev; 2018 Sep; 89(5):1613-1624. PubMed ID: 28378880
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Children's understanding that utterances emanate from minds: using speaker belief to aid interpretation.
    Mitchell P; Robinson EJ; Thompson DE
    Cognition; 1999 Aug; 72(1):45-66. PubMed ID: 10520564
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The road to understanding is paved with the speaker's intentions: cues to the speaker's attention and intentions affect pronoun comprehension.
    Nappa R; Arnold JE
    Cogn Psychol; 2014 May; 70():58-81. PubMed ID: 24534295
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The role of speaker eye gaze and mutual exclusivity in novel word learning by monolingual and bilingual children.
    Gangopadhyay I; Kaushanskaya M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Sep; 197():104878. PubMed ID: 32580087
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Words are not enough: how preschoolers' integration of perspective and emotion informs their referential understanding.
    Graham SA; San Juan V; Khu M
    J Child Lang; 2017 May; 44(3):500-526. PubMed ID: 27817761
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Did you or I say pretty, rude or brief? An ERP study of the effects of speaker's identity on emotional word processing.
    Pinheiro AP; Rezaii N; Nestor PG; Rauber A; Spencer KM; Niznikiewicz M
    Brain Lang; 2016 Feb; 153-154():38-49. PubMed ID: 26894680
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. A bilingual advantage in 54-month-olds' use of referential cues in fast mapping.
    Yow WQ; Li X; Lam S; Gliga T; Chong YS; Kwek K; Broekman BF
    Dev Sci; 2017 Jan; 20(1):. PubMed ID: 27882635
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. On how the brain decodes vocal cues about speaker confidence.
    Jiang X; Pell MD
    Cortex; 2015 May; 66():9-34. PubMed ID: 25796072
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Use of Speaker's Gaze and Syntax in Verb Learning.
    Nappa R; Wessel A; McEldoon KL; Gleitman LR; Trueswell JC
    Lang Learn Dev; 2009; 5(4):203-234. PubMed ID: 24465183
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Young children's understanding of the say-mean distinction in referential speech.
    Mitchell P; Russell J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1989 Jun; 47(3):467-90. PubMed ID: 2738516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Referent selection in children with Autism Spectrum Condition and intellectual disabilities: Do social cues affect word-to-object or word-to-location mappings?
    Field C; Lewis C; Allen ML
    Res Dev Disabil; 2019 Aug; 91():103425. PubMed ID: 31252178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Autistic adults anticipate and integrate meaning based on the speaker's voice: Evidence from eye-tracking and event-related potentials.
    Barzy M; Black J; Williams D; Ferguson HJ
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2020 Jun; 149(6):1097-1115. PubMed ID: 31714095
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. "That's really clever!" Ironic hyperbole understanding in children.
    Aguert M; LE Vallois C; Martel K; Laval V
    J Child Lang; 2018 Jan; 45(1):260-272. PubMed ID: 28462762
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Lexical and prosodic cues in the comprehension of relative certainty.
    Moore C; Harris L; Patriquin M
    J Child Lang; 1993 Feb; 20(1):153-67. PubMed ID: 8454680
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. That's not what you said earlier: preschoolers expect partners to be referentially consistent.
    Graham SA; Sedivy J; Khu M
    J Child Lang; 2014 Jan; 41(1):34-50. PubMed ID: 23398907
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.