These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

143 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28779389)

  • 21. Concept of uprightness in baboons: assessment with pictures of realistic scenes.
    Fagot J; Bonté E; Parron C
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):369-79. PubMed ID: 18925421
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Age effect in expert cognitive flexibility in Guinea baboons (Papio papio).
    Gullstrand J; Claidière N; Fagot J
    Behav Brain Res; 2022 Sep; 434():114043. PubMed ID: 35933047
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Analogical reasoning in baboons (Papio papio): flexible reencoding of the source relation depending on the target relation.
    Fagot J; Maugard A
    Learn Behav; 2013 Sep; 41(3):229-37. PubMed ID: 23354955
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. The transfer of category knowledge by macaques (Macaca mulatta) and humans (Homo sapiens).
    Zakrzewski AC; Church BA; Smith JD
    J Comp Psychol; 2018 Feb; 132(1):58-74. PubMed ID: 29239647
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Learned predictiveness influences rapid attentional capture: evidence from the dot probe task.
    Le Pelley ME; Vadillo M; Luque D
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2013 Nov; 39(6):1888-900. PubMed ID: 23855549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Associative learning in baboons (Papio papio) and humans (Homo sapiens): species differences in learned attention to visual features.
    Fagot J; Kruschke JK; Dépy D; Vauclair J
    Anim Cogn; 1998 Oct; 1(2):123-33. PubMed ID: 24399276
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Temporal Proximity Promotes Integration of Overlapping Events.
    Zeithamova D; Preston AR
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2017 Aug; 29(8):1311-1323. PubMed ID: 28253077
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Use of human visual attention cues by olive baboons (Papio anubis) in a competitive task.
    Vick SJ; Anderson JR
    J Comp Psychol; 2003 Jun; 117(2):209-16. PubMed ID: 12856791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Automated testing of cognitive performance in monkeys: use of a battery of computerized test systems by a troop of semi-free-ranging baboons (Papio papio).
    Fagot J; Bonté E
    Behav Res Methods; 2010 May; 42(2):507-16. PubMed ID: 20479182
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Transfer between local and global processing levels by pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) in exemplar- and rule-based categorization tasks.
    Aust U; Braunöder E
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Feb; 129(1):1-16. PubMed ID: 25150965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Humans (Homo sapiens) but not baboons (Papio papio) demonstrate crossmodal pitch-luminance correspondence.
    Margiotoudi K; Fagot J; Meguerditchian A; Dautriche I
    Am J Primatol; 2024 May; 86(5):e23613. PubMed ID: 38475662
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Baboons, like humans, solve analogy by categorical abstraction of relations.
    Flemming TM; Thompson RK; Fagot J
    Anim Cogn; 2013 May; 16(3):519-24. PubMed ID: 23334572
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. The Temporal Dynamics of Regularity Extraction in Non-Human Primates.
    Minier L; Fagot J; Rey A
    Cogn Sci; 2016 May; 40(4):1019-30. PubMed ID: 26303229
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. A comparative analysis of the categorization of multidimensional stimuli: I. Unidimensional classification does not necessarily imply analytic processing; evidence from pigeons (Columba livia), squirrels (Sciurus carolinensis), and humans (Homo sapiens).
    Wills AJ; Lea SE; Leaver LA; Osthaus B; Ryan CM; Suret MB; Bryant CM; Chapman SJ; Millar L
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Nov; 123(4):391-405. PubMed ID: 19929108
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Baboons' response speed is biased by their moods.
    Marzouki Y; Gullstrand J; Goujon A; Fagot J
    PLoS One; 2014; 9(7):e102562. PubMed ID: 25061682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Regularity Extraction Across Species: Associative Learning Mechanisms Shared by Human and Non-Human Primates.
    Rey A; Minier L; Malassis R; Bogaerts L; Fagot J
    Top Cogn Sci; 2019 Jul; 11(3):573-586. PubMed ID: 29785844
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Representation of serial order in pigeons (Columba livia).
    Scarf D; Colombo M
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2010 Oct; 36(4):423-9. PubMed ID: 20973610
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Comparison of grouping abilities in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio) with the Ebbinghaus illusion.
    Parron C; Fagot J
    J Comp Psychol; 2007 Nov; 121(4):405-11. PubMed ID: 18085924
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. The effects of item familiarity on the neural correlates of successful associative memory encoding.
    Dennis NA; Turney IC; Webb CE; Overman AA
    Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci; 2015 Dec; 15(4):889-900. PubMed ID: 25939781
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Evidence for compositionality in baboons (Papio papio) through the test case of negation.
    Dautriche I; Buccola B; Berthet M; Fagot J; Chemla E
    Sci Rep; 2022 Nov; 12(1):19181. PubMed ID: 36357450
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.