These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

137 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28883523)

  • 1. Information seeking about tool properties in great apes.
    Bohn M; Allritz M; Call J; Völter CJ
    Sci Rep; 2017 Sep; 7(1):10923. PubMed ID: 28883523
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Prior experience mediates the usage of food items as tools in great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo abelii).
    Ebel SJ; Völter CJ; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2021 Feb; 135(1):64-73. PubMed ID: 32463250
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Comparing the sniffing behavior of great apes.
    Jänig S; Weiß BM; Widdig A
    Am J Primatol; 2018 Jun; 80(6):e22872. PubMed ID: 29756687
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii) follow visual trails to locate hidden food.
    Völter CJ; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 May; 128(2):199-208. PubMed ID: 24866009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Chimpanzees show some evidence of selectively acquiring information by using tools, making inferences, and evaluating possible outcomes.
    Perdue BM; Evans TA; Beran MJ
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(4):e0193229. PubMed ID: 29641519
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Orangutans (Pongo abelii) seek information about tool functionality in a metacognition tubes task.
    Mulcahy NJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2016 Nov; 130(4):391-399. PubMed ID: 27841455
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Apes (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, P. troglodytes, Pongo abelii) versus corvids (Corvus corax, C. corone) in a support task: the effect of pattern and functionality.
    Albiach-Serrano A; Bugnyar T; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Nov; 126(4):355-67. PubMed ID: 22545765
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Can grey parrots (Psittacus erithacus) succeed on a "complex" foraging task failed by nonhuman primates (Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii, Sapajus apella) but solved by wrasse fish (Labroides dimidiatus)?
    Pepperberg IM; Hartsfield LA
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Aug; 128(3):298-306. PubMed ID: 24798239
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Comparative chemical analysis of body odor in great apes.
    Jänig S; Weiß BM; Birkemeyer C; Widdig A
    Am J Primatol; 2019 Jul; 81(6):e22976. PubMed ID: 31094019
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Information seeking by orangutans: a generalized search strategy?
    Marsh HL; MacDonald SE
    Anim Cogn; 2012 May; 15(3):293-304. PubMed ID: 21918872
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Differences in between-reinforcer value modulate the selective-value effect in great apes (Pan troglodytes, P. Paniscus, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo abelii).
    Sánchez-Amaro A; Peretó M; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2016 Feb; 130(1):1-12. PubMed ID: 26460854
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus, Pongo abelii) exploit better the information of failure than capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.) when selecting tools to solve the same foraging problem.
    Manrique HM; Call J; Visalberghi E; Sabbatini G
    J Comp Psychol; 2021 May; 135(2):273-279. PubMed ID: 33492157
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Naïve orangutans (Pongo abelii and Pongo pygmaeus) individually acquire nut-cracking using hammer tools.
    Bandini E; Grossmann J; Funk M; Albiach-Serrano A; Tennie C
    Am J Primatol; 2021 Sep; 83(9):e23304. PubMed ID: 34378813
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii) understand connectivity in the skewered grape tool task.
    Mulcahy NJ; Schubiger MN; Suddendorf T
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Feb; 127(1):109-13. PubMed ID: 22686164
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The interplay of prior experience and motivation in great ape problem-solving (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, and Pongo abelii).
    Ebel SJ; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2018 Aug; 132(3):294-305. PubMed ID: 29708360
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Differences in novel food response between Pongo and Pan.
    Forss SIF; Motes-Rodrigo A; Hrubesch C; Tennie C
    Am J Primatol; 2019 Jan; 81(1):e22945. PubMed ID: 30604887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Are great apes able to reason from multi-item samples to populations of food items?
    Eckert J; Rakoczy H; Call J
    Am J Primatol; 2017 Oct; 79(10):. PubMed ID: 28877364
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The limits of endowment effects in great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, Pongo pygmaeus).
    Kanngiesser P; Santos LR; Hood BM; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2011 Nov; 125(4):436-45. PubMed ID: 21767009
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The social dynamics of complex gestural communication in great and lesser apes (
    Amici F; Liebal K
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 2022 Sep; 377(1860):20210299. PubMed ID: 35934967
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. All great ape species (Gorilla gorilla, Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Pongo abelii) and two-and-a-half-year-old children (Homo sapiens) discriminate appearance from reality.
    Karg K; Schmelz M; Call J; Tomasello M
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Nov; 128(4):431-9. PubMed ID: 25150962
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.