These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

207 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 28899810)

  • 41. Quantity-based judgments in the domestic dog (Canis lupus familiaris).
    Ward C; Smuts BB
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Jan; 10(1):71-80. PubMed ID: 16941158
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Training differences predict dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) preferences for prosocial others.
    Silver ZA; Furlong EE; Johnston AM; Santos LR
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Jan; 24(1):75-83. PubMed ID: 32757105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Dogs (canis familiaris) underestimate the quantity of connected items: first demonstration of susceptibility to the connectedness illusion in non-human animals.
    Lõoke M; Marinelli L; Agrillo C; Guérineau C; Mongillo P
    Sci Rep; 2021 Dec; 11(1):23291. PubMed ID: 34857858
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Comparative analysis of the blood transcriptomes between wolves and dogs.
    Yang X; Zhang H; Shang J; Liu G; Xia T; Zhao C; Sun G; Dou H
    Anim Genet; 2018 Aug; 49(4):291-302. PubMed ID: 29953636
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Taxonomic status of the Australian dingo: the case for Canis dingo Meyer, 1793.
    Smith BP; Cairns KM; Adams JW; Newsome TM; Fillios M; Déaux EC; Parr WCH; Letnic M; VAN Eeden LM; Appleby RG; Bradshaw CJA; Savolainen P; Ritchie EG; Nimmo DG; Archer-Lean C; Greenville AC; Dickman CR; Watson L; Moseby KE; Doherty TS; Wallach AD; Morrant DS; Crowther MS
    Zootaxa; 2019 Mar; 4564(1):zootaxa.4564.1.6. PubMed ID: 31716519
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. A simple reason for a big difference: wolves do not look back at humans, but dogs do.
    Miklósi A; Kubinyi E; Topál J; Gácsi M; Virányi Z; Csányi V
    Curr Biol; 2003 Apr; 13(9):763-6. PubMed ID: 12725735
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Learning and owner-stranger effects on interspecific communication in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 May; 81(1):44-9. PubMed ID: 19429195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Oslerus osleri (metastrongyloidea; filaroididae) in gray wolves (Canis lupus) from Banff National Park, Alberta, Canada.
    Verocai GG; Schock DM; Lejeune M; Warren AL; Duignan PJ; Kutz SJ
    J Wildl Dis; 2013 Apr; 49(2):422-6. PubMed ID: 23568921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. A protected area influences genotype-specific survival and the structure of a Canis hybrid zone.
    Benson JF; Patterson BR; Mahoney PJ
    Ecology; 2014 Feb; 95(2):254-64. PubMed ID: 24669720
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Training Reduces Stress in Human-Socialised Wolves to the Same Degree as in Dogs.
    Vasconcellos Ada S; Virányi Z; Range F; Ades C; Scheidegger JK; Möstl E; Kotrschal K
    PLoS One; 2016; 11(9):e0162389. PubMed ID: 27611784
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Domestication Does Not Explain the Presence of Inequity Aversion in Dogs.
    Essler JL; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F
    Curr Biol; 2017 Jun; 27(12):1861-1865.e3. PubMed ID: 28602652
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. The performance of domestic dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) on two versions of the object choice task.
    Clark H; Leavens DA
    Anim Cogn; 2021 Sep; 24(5):1087-1098. PubMed ID: 33687599
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Wolves, but not dogs, are prosocial in a touch screen task.
    Dale R; Palma-Jacinto S; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(5):e0215444. PubMed ID: 31042740
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. The morphology of the mandibular coronoid process does not indicate that
    Janssens L; Miller R; Van Dongen S
    Zoomorphology; 2016; 135():269-277. PubMed ID: 27340333
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Tracking the evolutionary origins of dog-human cooperation: the "Canine Cooperation Hypothesis".
    Range F; Virányi Z
    Front Psychol; 2014; 5():1582. PubMed ID: 25642203
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Prevalence of Sarcocystis spp. in two subspecies of caribou (Rangifer tarandus) in Newfoundland and Labrador, and foxes (Vulpes vulpes), wolves (Canis lupus), and husky dogs (Canis familiaris) as potential definitive hosts.
    Khan RA; Evans L
    J Parasitol; 2006 Jun; 92(3):662-3. PubMed ID: 16884021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. The influence of relationships on neophobia and exploration in wolves and dogs.
    Moretti L; Hentrup M; Kotrschal K; Range F
    Anim Behav; 2015 Sep; 107():159-173. PubMed ID: 26405301
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Importance of a species' socioecology: Wolves outperform dogs in a conspecific cooperation task.
    Marshall-Pescini S; Schwarz JFL; Kostelnik I; Virányi Z; Range F
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2017 Oct; 114(44):11793-11798. PubMed ID: 29078337
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Wolves lead and dogs follow, but they both cooperate with humans.
    Range F; Marshall-Pescini S; Kratz C; Virányi Z
    Sci Rep; 2019 Mar; 9(1):3796. PubMed ID: 30846770
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Analytical validation of an Enzyme Immunoassay for the measurement of urinary oxytocin in dogs and wolves.
    Schaebs FS; Marshall-Pescini S; Range F; Deschner T
    Gen Comp Endocrinol; 2019 Sep; 281():73-82. PubMed ID: 31121163
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.