These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

165 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29294199)

  • 1. Irrational choice behavior in human and nonhuman primates.
    Perdue BM; Brown ER
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Mar; 21(2):227-234. PubMed ID: 29294199
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. No evidence of the choice overload effect in a computerized paradigm with rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella).
    Englund MD; Beran MJ
    Behav Processes; 2022 Jan; 194():104545. PubMed ID: 34800607
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Do monkeys choose to choose?
    Perdue BM; Evans TA; Washburn DA; Rumbaugh DM; Beran MJ
    Learn Behav; 2014 Jun; 42(2):164-75. PubMed ID: 24567075
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Hand preference for a bimanual task in tufted capuchins (Cebus apella) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).
    Westergaard GC; Suomi SJ
    J Comp Psychol; 1996 Dec; 110(4):406-11. PubMed ID: 8956511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Monkeys exhibit human-like gaze biases in economic decisions.
    Lupkin SM; McGinty VB
    Elife; 2023 Jul; 12():. PubMed ID: 37497784
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Personality structure in brown capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella): comparisons with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), orangutans (Pongo spp.), and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).
    Morton FB; Lee PC; Buchanan-Smith HM; Brosnan SF; Thierry B; Paukner A; de Waal FB; Widness J; Essler JL; Weiss A
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Aug; 127(3):282-98. PubMed ID: 23668695
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Looking ahead? Computerized maze task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), and human children (Homo sapiens).
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE; Futch SE; Evans TA; Perdue BM
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 May; 129(2):160-73. PubMed ID: 25798793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Do primates see the solitaire illusion differently? A comparative assessment of humans (Homo sapiens), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Agrillo C; Parrish AE; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Nov; 128(4):402-13. PubMed ID: 25133464
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Conceptual thresholds for same and different in old-(Macaca mulatta) and new-world (Cebus apella) monkeys.
    Flemming TM
    Behav Processes; 2011 Mar; 86(3):316-22. PubMed ID: 21238555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. State-dependent risky choices in primates: Variation in energy budget does not affect tufted capuchin monkeys' (Sapajus spp.) risky choices.
    Ciacci F; Mayerhoff S; De Petrillo F; Gastaldi S; Brosnan SF; Addessi E
    Am J Primatol; 2023 Oct; 85(10):e23542. PubMed ID: 37545247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. What are my chances? Closing the gap in uncertainty monitoring between rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2014 Jul; 40(3):303-16. PubMed ID: 25368870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) succeed in a test of quantity conservation.
    Beran MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):109-16. PubMed ID: 17549530
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. (Ir)rational choices of humans, rhesus macaques, and capuchin monkeys in dynamic stochastic environments.
    Watzek J; Brosnan SF
    Cognition; 2018 Sep; 178():109-117. PubMed ID: 29852425
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modulate their use of an uncertainty response depending on risk.
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Church BA; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2016 Jan; 42(1):32-43. PubMed ID: 26551351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Summation and quantity judgments of sequentially presented sets by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Evans TA; Leighty KA; Harris EH; Rice D
    Am J Primatol; 2008 Feb; 70(2):191-4. PubMed ID: 17879377
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Capuchin and rhesus monkeys show sunk cost effects in a psychomotor task.
    Watzek J; Brosnan SF
    Sci Rep; 2020 Nov; 10(1):20396. PubMed ID: 33230238
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Do tufted capuchin monkeys play the odds? Flexible risk preferences in Sapajus spp.
    De Petrillo F; Ventricelli M; Ponsi G; Addessi E
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jan; 18(1):119-30. PubMed ID: 24993065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Ordinal representation of numeric quantities by brown capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Judge PG; Evans TA; Vyas DK
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Jan; 31(1):79-94. PubMed ID: 15656729
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Information seeking by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Smith JD
    Cognition; 2011 Jul; 120(1):90-105. PubMed ID: 21459372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Do you see what I see? A comparative investigation of the Delboeuf illusion in humans (Homo sapiens), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Parrish AE; Brosnan SF; Beran MJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2015 Oct; 41(4):395-405. PubMed ID: 26322505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.