These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

212 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 29442250)

  • 21. The performance of bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) in two versions of an object-choice task.
    Mulcahy NJ; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Aug; 123(3):304-9. PubMed ID: 19685972
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Movement and vocal intonation together evoke social referencing in companion dogs when confronted with a suspicious stranger.
    Salamon A; Száraz J; Miklósi Á; Gácsi M
    Anim Cogn; 2020 Sep; 23(5):913-924. PubMed ID: 32500293
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Virányi Z; Gácsi M; Kubinyi E; Topál J; Belényi B; Ujfalussy D; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jul; 11(3):373-87. PubMed ID: 18183437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Domesticated dogs (Canis familiaris) tend to follow repeated deceptive human cues even when food is visible.
    Dwyer C; Cole MR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):442-448. PubMed ID: 30251105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Interpretation of human pointing by African elephants: generalisation and rationality.
    Smet AF; Byrne RW
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Nov; 17(6):1365-74. PubMed ID: 24942107
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Is that fear? Domestic dogs' use of social referencing signals from an unfamiliar person.
    Yong MH; Ruffman T
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():74-81. PubMed ID: 25245302
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners.
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Barrera G; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):402-8. PubMed ID: 19520240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. The order of ostensive and referential signals affects dogs' responsiveness when interacting with a human.
    Tauzin T; Csík A; Kis A; Kovács K; Topál J
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Jul; 18(4):975-9. PubMed ID: 25771965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Physical size matters in the domestic dog's (Canis lupus familiaris) ability to use human pointing cues.
    Helton WS; Helton ND
    Behav Processes; 2010 Sep; 85(1):77-9. PubMed ID: 20553825
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. A comparative approach to dogs' (Canis familiaris) and human infants' comprehension of various forms of pointing gestures.
    Lakatos G; Soproni K; Dóka A; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jul; 12(4):621-31. PubMed ID: 19343382
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. The Role of Life History and Familiarity in Performance of Working and Non-Working Dogs (
    McCartney JM; Leavens DA
    Animals (Basel); 2024 Feb; 14(4):. PubMed ID: 38396541
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Social referencing in dog-owner dyads?
    Merola I; Prato-Previde E; Marshall-Pescini S
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):175-85. PubMed ID: 21874515
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task?
    Kupán K; Miklósi Á; Gergely G; Topál J
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Mar; 14(2):259-68. PubMed ID: 21140185
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Selective responding to human ostensive communication is an early developing capacity of domestic dogs.
    Byosiere SE; Mundry R; Range F; Virányi Z
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13361. PubMed ID: 36545915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Are dogs (Canis familiaris) misled more by their owners than by strangers in a food choice task?
    Marshall-Pescini S; Prato-Previde E; Valsecchi P
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Jan; 14(1):137-42. PubMed ID: 20644973
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Dogs' (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures.
    Soproni K; Miklósi A; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2002 Mar; 116(1):27-34. PubMed ID: 11926681
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Do dogs (Canis lupus familiaris) make counterproductive choices because they are sensitive to human ostensive cues?
    Marshall-Pescini S; Passalacqua C; Miletto Petrazzini ME; Valsecchi P; Prato-Previde E
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(4):e35437. PubMed ID: 22558150
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Adjustment in the point-following behaviour of free-ranging dogs - roles of social petting and informative-deceptive nature of cues.
    Bhattacharjee D; Bhadra A
    Anim Cogn; 2022 Jun; 25(3):571-579. PubMed ID: 34743236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Use of experimenter-given cues by African gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus).
    Giret N; Miklósi A; Kreutzer M; Bovet D
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):1-10. PubMed ID: 18543008
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.
    Kirchhofer KC; Zimmermann F; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(2):e30913. PubMed ID: 22347411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.